RFT

RFT

 

Relational Frame Theory (RFT) is an explicitly psychological account of human language and cognition. It is an approach designed to be a pragmatically useful analysis of complex human behavior, and provides the empirical and conceptual tools to conduct an experimental analysis of virtually every substantive topic in this arena. Further, the contextual approach of RFT provides a functional account of the structure of verbal knowledge and cognition, creating an important link between the traditionally disparate perspectives of cognitive and behavioral psychology.

While there are many different theories of language and cognition available, RFT offers several distinct advantages over traditional approaches. We believe these advantages make RFT of interest not only to behavioral theorists but also to cognitive psychologists, therapists, educators, and anyone studying the human condition. To learn more about RFT, click on a link below.

Community

About RFT

About RFT

This is where you can read about the basic principles in Relational Frame Theory and learn about its advantages over other theories of language and cognition.

What is RFT?

Advantages of RFT

For suggestions on further reading, please visit Resources for Learning RFT. To learn more about RFT research and application, please visit the RFT Research section.

 

Community

What is RFT?

What is RFT?

There is a strong empirical and conceptual relationship between language and derived stimulus relations. An empirical relationship does not indicate that derived stimulus relations depend upon language or that such relations are mediated by language. When two dependent variables are correlated, one conservative strategy is to determine whether both variables are reflective of the same basic underlying psychological process. If the two areas do overlap at the level of behavioral process, then questions about human language may also be questions about derived stimulus relations, and vice versa.

This is the basic theoretical and empirical research strategy of RFT. The overarching aim of this behavioral research has been to integrate a range of apparently diverse psychological phenomena including, for example, stimulus equivalence, naming, understanding, analogy, metaphor, and rule-following.

Relational Frame Theory adopts the view that the core defining element in all of these, and many other inherently verbal activities, is arbitrarily applicable relational responding, and moreover that such responding is amenable to a learning or operant analysis.

RFT treats relational responding as a generalized operant, and thus appeals to a history of multiple-exemplar training. Specific types of relational responding, termed relational frames, are defined in terms of the three properties of mutual and combinatorial entailment, and the transformation of functions. Relational frames are arbitrarily applicable, but are typically not necessarily arbitrarily applied in the natural language context.

Mutual entailment refers to the derived bidirectionality of some stimulus relations, and as such it is a generic term for the concept of "symmetry" in stimulus equivalence. "Mutual entailment" applies if stimulus A is related to another stimulus B in a specific context, and as a result a relation between B and A is entailed in that context. Combinatorial entailment refers to instances in which two or more relations that have acquired the property of mutual entailment mutually combine. Combinatorial entailment is the generic term for what is called "transitivity" and "equivalence" in stimulus equivalence. Combinatorial entailment applies when, in a given context, A is related to B and B is related to C, and then in that context a relation is entailed between A and C and another between C and A. For example, if A is bigger than B, and B is bigger than C, then a bigger-than relation is entailed between A and C, and a smaller-than relation is entailed between C and A. A transformation of stimulus functions applies when functions of one event in a relational network is altered based on the functions of another event in the network and the derived relation between them. Mutual and combinatorial entailment are regulated by contextual cues (C rel). The transformation of stimulus functions are regulated by additional contextual cues (C func).

The development of relational responding can be organized into a rough list that gradually becomes more and more complex. We are not presenting this list as a set of stages or steps, and we would expect them to be sequenced only in broad terms and even then only if the training history is typical. Nevertheless, this list gives a sense of the complexity that emerges from the small set of core concepts in Relational Frame Theory.

  • Simple examples of verbal understanding
  • Contextually controlled mutual entailment in additional types of stimulus relations
  • Contextually controlled combinatorial entailment in additional types of stimulus relations
  • Contextually controlled transformation of stimulus functions in additional types of stimulus relations
  • Integration of these into additional relational frames
  • Simple examples of genuinely verbal governance of behavior by others
  • Conditional contextual control over the participation of given elements in relational frames
  • More complex examples of verbal understanding
  • Verbal governance of the behavior of others (e.g., verbal mands and tacts)
  • Transformation of stimulus functions across relational networks
  • Increasing acquisition of specific participants in specific relational frames (e.g., vocabulary)
  • Complex interactions between relations (training in one influences development of another)
  • Elaborated and increasingly subtle contextual control over relational responding (e.g., syntax; number of relational terms)
  • Nonarbitrary properties serve as a relational context for arbitrarily applicable relational responses
  • With acquisition of equivalence, time or causality, and evaluation, the development of relational sentences that function fully as rules
  • Relating relational networks
  • Transformation of stimulus functions based on the relating of relational networks
  • Relating relational networks under the control of nonarbitrary properties of the environment
  • Regulation of the behavior of the listener through the establishment of relational networks in the listener
  • With the acquisition of hierarchical class membership, use of relational networks to abstract nonarbitrary properties and to have these properties participate in relational frames
  • Abstracting properties of the nonarbitrary environment based on relational networks and the relating of relational networks
  • With the acquisition of temporal, contingency, and causal relational frames, increased insensitivity to temporal delays
  • Development of perspective-taking and sense of self
  • Construction of the verbal other
  • Construction of the conceptualized group
  • Contextual control of relational responding by the nonarbitrary and arbitrary properties of the listener
  • Regulation of the behavior of the listener by orienting the listener to abstracted features of the environment
  • Acquisition of increasingly abstract verbal consequences
  • Self-rule generation and self rule-following
  • Increasing dominance of the verbal functions of the environment

The foregoing provides a summary of the key features of RFT. The key concept that underlies Relational Frame Theory is extremely simple—try to think of relating per se as learned behavior. As the list above shows, however, applying this simple idea leads to many specific points—the nature of an arbitrarily applicable relational response, the role of context, the varieties of relational responses, the role of the nonarbitrary environment, networks of relations, the use of these abilities to solve problems, the development of self, and so on.

Steven Hayes

Advantages of RFT

Advantages of RFT

Advantages of the RFT Approach to Human Language and Cognition

There are many different theories—in many different disciplines—that attempt to explain or account for human language and cognition. With so many different theories available, what is unique or special about Relational Frame Theory?

We believe the functional, contextualistic approach of RFT to understanding complex human behavior has led to a system of analysis that offers many advantages over the traditional structural and “information transmission” models of language and cognition (Blackledge, 2003). These advantages include:

  • RFT is parsimonious, relying on relatively few basic principles and concepts to account for language and cognition.
  • RFT is precise, allowing the study of human language to be conducted in accordance with the carefully-specified definitions of its component processes.
  • RFT has broad scope, providing plausible explanations and new empirical approaches to a wide variety of complex human behaviors in both basic and applied domains (such as problem solving, metaphors, self, spirituality, values, rule-governed behavior, psychopathology, intelligence, etc.).
  • RFT has depth, meaning that its analyses cohere with established treatments at other levels of analysis. For example, it provides plausible accounts of cultural phenomena such as knowledge amplification; recent neurological research indicates that the brain processes seen while subjects engage in derived relational responding fit with the RFT language claim; and connectionist models of the learning history needed to establish relational frames coheres with RFT.
  • The principles of RFT are directly observable, especially under laboratory conditions, so no tenuous inferences about the existence of unseen structures or processes (such as cognitive schemas or language acquisition devices) are required.
  • RFT is firmly based on empirical research that has without exception supported its tenets. In addition to the over 30 published empirical treatments of RFT, the theory also accounts for the data observed in hundreds of empirical studies on the concept of stimulus equivalence that have been published since 1971. RFT has withstood all empirical tests so far, and all of its core claims now have at least some supportive data. So far, no data has arisen in contradiction to the theory.
  • RFT has direct applied and clinical applications that are not apparent in other accounts of human language and cognition. There are many successful empirical studies on applied methods based on RFT (particularly Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, but including other methods as well, such as methods of attitude change, or the treatment of stigma and prejudice, or increasing intelligence) and many clear applied implications yet to be pursued.
  • RFT is generative. The account leads quickly to innovative and (so far) empirically successful approaches to virtually all of the important topics in the language and cognition domain.
  • RFT is testable. Its core claim (that relating can be thought of as learned, operant behavior) is an empirical matter. If relational frames do not develop, come under contextual control, respond to shaping via multiple exemplar training, and respond to consequences, then the theory is false. Further, its claim that relational frames are the core of human language is testable both directly and pragmatically. For example, if RFT does not lead to more successful education interventions than those that currently exist, then it fails. (see section on research evidence).
  • RFT is progressive. RFT supports what is within the "protective belt" of the behavioral paradigm and yet is generative in the sense described above (see Lakatos for this approach to progressivity). RFT is a behavioral theory that builds on everything that is known about basic behavioral principles, but takes this basic account into a fundamentally new direction with profound and exciting implications for almost every topic relating to complex human behavior. Yet it does so without any patchwork corrections to the basic assumptions of the behavioral paradigm. This has resulted in newer models that are being updated as well as changes in methodology to study topics of interest more precisely.
  • RFT is coherent. Its philosophical basis is well articulated; its assumptions are clearly stated; its concepts are carefully defined; and all of these levels fit together.
Eric Fox

Resources for Learning RFT

Resources for Learning RFT

We recommend several ways to enhance your understanding of RFT. A great place to start is with the Learning RFT: An introduction to relational frame theory and its clinical applications but there are many more.

Whether or not you intend to gain an in-depth understanding of RFT, we believe that having a basic understanding of RFT and contextual behavioral science can be invaluable for conducting clinical work. We do not believe that not having this knowledge is at all a detriment to clinical work, but we do know that when folks take the time to do so they report finding it illuminating and they simply do not think in the same way after learning about RFT.

We have compiled several resources for anyone with an interest in RFT, novice and expert alike:

  • those who are interested in understanding the principles as they inform clinical practice;
  • those who are interested in gaining a more in-depth understanding of the theoretical model and its research base;
  • and those who are seeking formal academic training in a laboratory conducting research on RFT and its principles.

thinkingcapwhoa_color.gif

If you have an interest in discussing RFT with others who share your interests, please consider joining the RFT email listserv no matter what your background and training in RFT. The community is interested in learning and growing together and your questions may just push the community to consider new ways of approaching the work. Like the main ACT for Professionals listserv, you must be a current paid member of ACBS prior to joining.

In the child pages below you will find reading suggestions, multimedia presentations on RFT principles and how they relate to clinical phenomena, suggestions for linking up with others to learn about RFT, and information on the newly improved and highly successful RFT tutorial and how to take it.

Jen Plumb

RFT Tutorial

RFT Tutorial

An Introduction to Relational Frame Theory is a fully online, self-paced, interactive, multimedia tutorial designed to introduce the basic concepts and approach of Relational Frame Theory (RFT). It was developed by Dr. Eric J. Fox and first released in 2004, and has been regularly updated and maintained by FoxyLearning since 2010. An open-access version can be completed for free, a CEU version can be completed at CEUniverse to earn 7 continuing education units to maintain certification as a Board Certified Behavior Analyst (BCBA), and instructors or trainers can assign a low-cost version of the free version as a supplement to their course or training. The tutorial was written and designed for a very broad audience, and helps users to gain mastery over complex concepts in RFT by breaking them down and allowing the user to practice them along the away. It is hoped that everyone from undergraduate psychology students to doctoral-level psychologists to any educated person on the street (or on the web!) will find the material accessible, engaging, and relevant.

In 2005 the tutorial received the Nova Southeastern Award for Outstanding Practice by a Graduate Student in Instructional Design from the Design & Development division of the Association for Educational Communications & Technology. With an award name that long, you know it's got to be good.

Visit FoxyLearning to learn more!

Eric Fox

Articles Linking RFT and ACT

Articles Linking RFT and ACT

There is an extensive list of excellent articles that describe RFT in simple terms, and links the principles directly to ACT processes. We highly recommend reading some of these to further your understanding RFT and it's relation to ACT.

Please visit the page by clicking here.

Jen Plumb

Suggested Readings & Helpful Presentations

Suggested Readings & Helpful Presentations

There are numerous resources for further reading. These are simply a few suggestions and you may find many others helpful. We highly recommend using this list as a starting point from which to begin your journey. We also recommend you click on this link to see a longer list of RFT/Behavior Analysis books and also to use the Publications list as an instrument for guiding your own path of learning.

Top 10 RFT Research Articles

In Spring 2023, the RFT SIG took on the task of developing an unofficial list of top RFT research articles to help people know where to start diving into the literature. The categories were “Just Getting Started” and “Contemporary and Advanced”.

Just Getting Started

  • Hayes, S. C., Law, S., Assemi, K., Falletta-Cowden, N., Shamblin, M., Burleigh, K., ... & Smith, P. (2021). Relating is an operant: A fly over of 35 years of RFT research. Perspectivas em Análise do Comportamento, 12(1), 5-32.
  • Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & O’Hora, D. (2010). Relational frame theory and human intelligence. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 11(1), 37-51.
  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Cullinan, V. (2000). Relational frame theory and Skinner’s Verbal Behavior: A possible synthesis. The Behavior Analyst, 23(1), 69-84.
  • Ming, S., Moran, L., & Stewart, I. (2014). Derived relational responding and generative language: Applications and future directions for teaching individuals with autism spectrum disorders. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 15(2), 199-224.
  • Blackledge, J. T. (2003). An introduction to relational frame theory: Basics and applications. The Behavior Analyst Today, 3(4), 421.
  • Stewart, I., McElwee, J., & Ming, S. (2013). Language generativity, response generalization, and derived relational responding. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 29(1), 137-155.
  • Pelaez, M., & Monlux, K. (2018). Development of communication in infants: Implications for stimulus relations research. Perspectives on Behavior Science, 41(1), 175-188.
  • McEnteggart, C. (2018). A brief tutorial on acceptance and commitment therapy as seen through the lens of derived stimulus relations. Perspectives on Behavior Science, 41(1), 215-227.
  • Healy, O., Barnes‐Holmes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (2000). Derived relational responding as generalized operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74(2), 207-227.
  • Stapleton, A., & McHugh, L. (2021). Healthy selfing: Theoretically optimal environments for the development of tacting and deictic relational responding. Perspectivas em Análise do Comportamento, 12(1), 125-137.

Contemporary and Advanced

  • Kirsten, E. B., & Stewart, I. (2021). Assessing the development of relational framing in young children. The Psychological Record, 72(1), 221-246.
  • Belisle, J., & Dixon, M. R. (2020). Relational density theory: Nonlinearity of equivalence relating examined through higher-order volumetric-mass-density. Perspectives on Behavior Science, 43(1), 259-283.
  • Cummins, J., Nevejans, M., Colbert, D., & De Houwer, J. (2023). On the structure of relational responding. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 27(1), 16-25.
  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & McEnteggart, C. (2020). Updating RFT (more field than frame) and its implications for process-based therapy. The Psychological Record, 70(1), 605-624.
  • Hayes, L. J., & Fryling, M. J. (2019). Functional and descriptive contextualism. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 14(1), 119-126.
  • Foody, M., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Törneke, N., Luciano, C., Stewart, I., & McEnteggart, C. (2014). RFT for clinical use: The example of metaphor. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 3(4), 305-313.
  • Delabie, M., Cummins, J., Finn, M., & De Houwer, J. (2022). Differential Crel and Cfunc acquisition through stimulus pairing. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 24(1), 112-119.
  • Mulhern, T., Stewart, I., & McElwee, J. (2018). Facilitating relational framing of classification in young children. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 8(1), 55-68.
  • Kirsten, E. B., Stewart, I., & McElwee, J. (2022). Testing and training analogical relational responding in children with and without autism. The Psychological Record, 72(1), 561-583.
  • Stewart, I., Barnes‐Holmes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. M. (2002). A functional‐analytic model of analogy: A relational frame analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78(3), 375-396. 

General Theory Books on RFT and Contextual Behavior Science

  • Villatte, M., Villatte, J. L., & Hayes, S. C. (2019). Mastering the clinical conversation: Language as intervention. New York: The Guilford Press.
  • Dahl, J., Stewart, I., Martell, C., Kaplan, J. (2014) ACT and RFT in Relationships: Helping Clients Deepen Intimacy and Maintain Healthy Commitments Using Acceptance and Commitment Therapy and Relational Frame Theory.
  • Dymond, S., & Roche, B. (Eds.) (2013). Advances in relational frame theory: Research and application. New Harbinger Publications.
  • Törneke, N. (2010). Learning RFT: An introduction to relational frame theory and its clinical applications. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications, Inc. 

    This book is designed to make RFT accessible to clinicians. This book is extremely readable and helps the reader understand behavioral principles, technical terms within RFT, and how to apply RFT across many different areas.

  • Ramnero, J., & Törneke, N. (2008). ABCs of human behavior: Behavioral principles for the practicing clinician. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger & Reno, NV: Context Press.

    The ABCs of Human Behavior offers the practicing clinician a solid and practical introduction to the basics of modern behavioral psychology. The book focuses both on the classical principles of learning as well as more recent developments that explain language and cognition in behavioral and contextual terms. These principles are not just discussed in the abstract—rather the book shows how the principles of learning apply in a clinical context. Practical and easy to read, the book walks you through both common sense and clinical examples that will help you use behavioral principles to observe, explain, and influence behavior in a therapeutic setting.

  • Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (Eds.). (2001). Relational Frame Theory: A Post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Plenum Press.

    This book may be one of the most difficult to read, but it is also the most thorough account of RFT principles and we highly recommend reading it (at some point) to gain a thorough and working understanding of RFT. Suggestion: do the RFT tutorial first. Read chapters 1 to 8, not stopping when you do not understand. Then pause and re-read Chapter 8. Then re-read the whole book and now you can stop and try to figure out what you do not understand. Don't worry. You will survive it.

  • Hayes, S. C. (Ed.). (1989/2004). Rule governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control. New York: Plenum / reprinted in 2004 by Context Press.

    One of the first full-length presentations of the ACT / RFT model is in three chapters in this book on the topic. This book is now available in paperback from Context Press.

Special Issues

Conceptual Developments in Relational Frame Theory: Research and Practice. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science.

Videos

admin

RFT Videos

RFT Videos Community

Advances in Relational Frame Theory: Research and Application -- Book Interview

Advances in Relational Frame Theory: Research and Application -- Book Interview

Edited by leading Relational Frame Theory (RFT) scholars, Simon Dymond, PhD, and Bryan Roche, PhD, Advances in Relational Frame Theory presents advances in all aspects of RFT research over the last decade, and provides a greater understanding of the core principals of acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT). The book also contains chapters written by Steven C. Hayes and Kelly Wilson, both research-active experts from the RFT community around the world.

Below, the book's editors Simon Dymond and Bryan Roche discuss the book, as well as the future of RFT.

Q: So what in a nutshell is Relational Frame Theory (RFT)?

Relational Frame Theory (RFT) is a contextual behavioral theory of language and cognition. It holds that the essence of language and cognitive thought is relational in nature.

Q: Why did you produce this book and why now?

As we outline in the Preface, it's been more than ten years since the publication of the first edited volume on RFT: the so-called, "purple book" (Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, & Roche, 2001).

Q: Who is the target audience?

Everyone with an interest in behaviour analysis, verbal behaviour, language and cognition, and clinical therapy. Really, there's something for everyone in the book, be they students, researchers, clinicians or just interested in what modern behaviour analysis has got to say about complex human behaviour.

Q: What might therapists, or other practitioners, and researchers get from the book?

RFT has truly emerged from a very healthy applied-basic research agenda. The theory has been informed from the beginning by the concerns of those trying to explain real world phenomena in the clinic, work or social setting. But it has also been built from the ground up based on sound principles of behavior analysis. So this theory speaks very well to therapists and practitioners in the work or clinical setting. RFT is not an esoteric theory – we always have our eye on the prize, which is not surprising given that we are pragmatists.

Several chapters in the book deal with clinical issues, and explain what RFT has to offer by way of well worked out functional explanations of clinical phenomena. These analysis have very real and immediate implications for the practice of therapy and show how RFT feeds directly into clinical practice. A further chapter illustrates the application of RFT to understanding behavior and behavior change in the workplace. These chapters will help practitioners get a grip on how it is precisely that RFT underpins therapeutic practices such as those used in Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) and other mindfulness based treatments. It is an excellent primer for a practitioner who wants a one-stop-shop for everything you needed to know about ACT and RFT.

But researchers will also get a lot from this book. Several chapters outline the verbal/relational underpinnings of clinical problems, which has the potential to better inform novel therapeutic interventions and to inspire exciting translational research endeavours. It represents the latest compendium of advances made in basic RFT research this past decade and is an essential book for any basic researchers interested in cutting edge changes in the experimental analysis of language and cognition. In other words, it's sssential reading for all those interested in clinical issues!

Q: What are some highlights of the book?

That's a hard question; there are so many highlights! Perhaps one of the most exciting advances made in RFT in recent years is its interfacing with neuroscience. The use of fMRI and EEG measures have complimented the development of RFT and some very impressive studies and analyses have been conducted since the publication of the first RFT book in 2001. These studies allow us to speak directly to neuroscientists and to validate some of the predictions of RFT regarding the verbal nature of the transformation of functions and to identify the neural correlates of language and cognitive phenomena in behavioral terms. Many readers will also be keen to read the chapter on the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP), which has also been developed since the 2001 RFT book. The IRAP uses an RFT approach to implicit testing to develop a test that allows researchers to measure implicit attitudes with an empirical integrity that probably outshines that achieved by the eponymous Implicit Association Test. This an excellent example of the application of RFT in domains of research (in this case social cognition) that were typically avoided by behavior analysts. It also represents an excellent example of bridge-building across the various domains of psychology and an impressive example of the kinds of translational research behavior analysts need to be doing.

We should also point out that there is an important conceptual chapter in the book in which the approach to science that we call contextual behavioral science (CBS), is integrated into a larger body of selectionist and contextualistic scientific approaches, namely evolution theory. This is an important new illustration of the fact that contextual behavioral science is quite an all-encompassing approach to science that can move us closer to a single, but multi-level and unified account of individual’s behavior. These types of conceptual advances are very exciting and very important for assessing the maturity and progressiveness of the CBS approach.

But, there are 11 chapters in the book, and all are worth highlighting!

Q: What are the future challenges and opportunities for RFT?

RFT is still a young theory in terms of the number of basic researchers who employ it as a paradigm in their basic research. It has come a long way but we need to see more translational work being done so that researchers and practitioners in other fields sit up and take notice. This has already started to happen in the fields of experimental psychopathology and implicit testing, and of course it has already happened for ACT in the world of therapy. 

Numerous challenges lie ahead, such as ensuring new organisations like ACBS and journals such as JCBS are able to continue to "spread the word" about RFT to a wider audience, especially given ongoing changes in scholarly publishing models and the growth of open access solutions. This is balanced, however, by numerous emerging opportunities such as the widespread acceptance that verbal/relational processes may play an important role in causing and maintaining human suffering (and the potential for RFT to inform such clinical theories), the fact that RFT is uniquely positioned to contribute its own functional account of language and cognition to debates underway in cognitive science and clinical psychology (Jan De Houwer, in the Foreword to the book, makes a great point in this regard), and the momentum behind ACBS and other organisations in reaching out and developing a genuine community of researchers, clinicians and students interested in RFT and its implications. We think the best service we can do for behavior analysis is to showcase RFT as our crowning achievement to date, and to do our best to use it to bring a behavior-analytic perspective to all and any behavior, regardless of their apparent level of complexity.


 

Douglas Long

Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) Website

Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) Website

Web Address (URL):

Open Source IRAP

An implementation of the IRAP is now freely available in open source form, running in PsychoPy (www.psychopy.org), and is now compatible with OSX, Windows, and Linux platforms at the above address. The original implementation of the IRAP in Visual Basic 6 can still downloaded using the attachment on the bottom of this page.

Description:

The IRAP was built out of RFT, the central postulate of which is that higher-cognitive functioning is composed of relational acts. Initial studies have shown that the IRAP may be used to measure relational networks or beliefs that are not readily accessible to the researcher or perhaps even the participant. The IRAP appears to offer advantages over other methods that use reaction time measures to assess beliefs or attitudes (e.g. the Implicit Association Test; IAT), both in its theoretical rationale and its ability to measure many types of relationships. This website provides an introduction to the IRAP research programme and access to the IRAP software and supporting materials.
 

Community

Suggested Readings pre-2010

Suggested Readings pre-2010

Understanding RFT: A few simple articles and presentations

  • Blackledge, J. T. (2003). An introduction to Relational Frame Theory: Basics and applications. The Behavior Analyst Today, 3(4), 421-433.
  • Blackledge, J.T. & Moran, D.J. (2009). Introduction to relational frame theory for clinicians. Japanese version published in Kokoro no Rinsho.
  • Törneke, N. & Luoma, J. (2009, July). RFT: Basic concepts and clinical implications. Paper presented at the 3rd World Conference for Contextual Behavioral Science, Enschede, Netherlands.
  • Blackledge, J. T. (2009). How is RFT Relevant to Clinical Psychology? Audio and handouts from a teleconference given to the Psychotherapy Research Network in March, 2009.

 

The role of RFT in Contextual Behavioral Science

 

Reviews & Situating RFT in the Larger Literature

  • Gross, A. C., & Fox, E. J. (2009). Relational frame theory: An overview of the controversy. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 25, 87-98.
  • Vilardaga, R. (2009, May). A systematic review of the RFT literature on deictic framing. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Association of Behavior Analysis International conference, Phoenix, AZ.

 

ACT/RFT Reader Update

  • ACT/RFT Reader Update is a review by CBS reseachers of recent research in both ACT and RFT that was published from 2018 - 2011. Each of the articles reviewed is available on this site.

 

Books on Applying RFT

     Derived Relational Responding offers a series of revolutionary intervention programs for applied work in human language and cognition targeted at students with autism and other developmental disabilities. It presents a program drawn from derived stimulus relations that you can use to help students of all ages acquire foundational and advanced verbal, social, and cognitive skills.

     This volume presents a contemporary behavioral model of behavior disorders that incorporates the findings of current RFT and ACT research. Rich in possibilities for clinical work, this view of disordered behavior is an important milestone in clinical psychotherapy - an opportunity for behavioral clinicians to reintegrate their clinical practice with an experimental analysis of behavior.

     This book is an applied volume in purpose, but includes an RFT account of each of the ACT processes, and in particular an in depth RFT perspective on personal values and the clinical interventions employed to enhance them and promote committed action.

     While not explicitly a volume on RFT, this book is an excellent resource on clinical behavioral approaches to common problems and includes several chapters with an RFT perspective on clinical problems.

 

News & Other Media

 

Study Guides for RFT Books

 

Community

Study Groups for RFT Books

Study Groups for RFT Books

Community

Learning RFT (the book) Study Group - Winter 2010/2011

Learning RFT (the book) Study Group - Winter 2010/2011

LearningRFTbookcover.jpgInterest in Niklas Torneke's new book Learning RFT has spawned a discussion group on the listservs.

Here, Joe Parsons has generously supplied a Powerpoint for getting folks started on Chapter 1.

You'll need to be logged into your current, paid ACBS member account to view/download it.  (Can't remember if your membership is current? Login then click on "Dashboard" on the right, then see your membership expiration date on the right side of the screen.)

Joe Parsons

Relational Frame Theory (the book) Study Group for Beginners - 2006

Relational Frame Theory (the book) Study Group for Beginners - 2006
PLEASE NOTE: This study group is not longer running, but we have left the information gathered here for your perusal. You may find the information in these pages of some use to you. Also, you may consider starting your own study group by asking colleagues and others on the listservs of their interests and then using these pages or asking ACBS staff to help you update them for your current purposes.

This is a place for people who are perhaps not behaviourally trained to learn RFT. We are primarily a group of clinicians and others who have been drawn to RFT through our exposure to ACT. Together, with each other's help, we are walking through the Relational Frame Theory: A Post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition book chapter by chapter and discussing both our understandings and our struggles. Please join in if you like. Beginning June 2006, our plan is to read one chapter a month, commenting on it as we go. We are particularly open to people who may know more than we do. So if you read something here that seems as if we're barking up the wrong conceptual tree, please, don't hold back. For those who are participating, let's try to remember that the only stupid question is the one not asked.

Guidelines for Posting/Contributing

There are two basic ways to contribute to our ongoing discussion: by adding child pages or by adding comments. The differences between these are described below. Child Pages Child pages are used to create entirely new web pages that are connected to a "parent" page. For example, this page that you're reading is a child page of the "About RFT" page (likewise, the "About RFT" page is the "parent page" of this page). "RFT Book Summary & Discussion" is a child page of this page. You can add a child page to any existing page by clicking on the "add child page" link at the bottom of the page. When you add a child page, several things happen:

  1. A new web page is created with whatever title and content you give it
  2. A link to your new page will be listed at the bottom of the parent page (like the link to "RFT Book Summary & Discussion" seen at the bottom of this page)
  3. A link to your new page will appear in the menu hierarchy on the left side of the screen (like the link to "RFT Book Summary & Discussion" seen on the left side of the screen below "RFT Study Group for Beginners"

So when should use add a child page? When you are contributing a new summary or question or discussion point. If you are just responding to something someone else has already posted, you should add a comment to their page (see below). For example, if you wanted to add a summary of Chapter 4 of the RFT book, you would go to the Chapter 4 page (RFT > About RFT > RFT Study Group for Beginners > RFT Book & Discussion > Chapter 4) and then click on the "add child page" link at the bottom of that page. A link to your summary page would then appear at the bottom of the Chapter 4 page and below Chapter 4 in the hierarchical menu on the left. Comments Unlike a child page, a comment is not a new web page. It is simply a comment added to the bottom of an existing page. Each page can have an unlimited number of comments, and users can reply to existing comments. In this way, every page can be like a whole discussion board with a primary post (the "child page" that has been added) and a discussion listed below it that consists of a series of comments. If you are just responding to something someone else has already posted, you should probably just add a comment to the page by clicking on the "add new comment" link at the bottom of that page. If you are responding to an existing comment, you can click on the "reply" link listed in the comment itself. Happy commenting, child paging, discussing, and learning!

jsteinwachs

RFT Book Summary & Discussion

RFT Book Summary & Discussion

This section is for individuals to offer their summaries, questions, and comments about the Relational Frame Theory book.

Eric Fox

Chapter 1

Chapter 1
This page is for summaries, discussions, and questions about Chapter 1 of the RFT book.
  • To add a new summary, question, or discussion item, click on the add a child page link at the bottom of this page. This will create a new web page for your summary or question. It will also create a link to your new page at the bottom of this page.
  • To respond to a summary, question, or discussion item someone has already posted, go to the page that contains their question or summary and click on the add new comment link at the bottom of that page. This will append your comment to the bottom of that page (it does not create a new web page). You can also reply to existing comments by clicking on the "reply" link in the comment.
Happy commenting, child paging, discussing, and learning!
Eric Fox

1.3.1 Rule governed behaviour

1.3.1 Rule governed behaviour

What's hardest for me in all of this is learning the language. It's very precise, and not intuitive for me. Okay, enough whining.
Questions:

1. Autoclitic frames?
Autoclitic: a unit of verbal behaviour that depends on other verbal behaviour for its occurrence and that modifies the effects of that behaviour on the listener. (Catania)
Ex:if-then?
So in Skinner's quote on pg 15 he's pointing to relational framing as a behaviour without explicitly delineating it?

2. Language hypothesis-the idea that differences between instructed and uninstructed performances could be accounted for by human language.
...the behaviour is verbal in Skinner's approach because a specially conditioned listener mediates reinforcement of this behaviour. (p.16)
I'm still struggling with this. So, for Skinner, if a tree falls in the forest and no one hears it, it doesn't make a sound?
Another question: For Skinner, can the listener and the speaker be the same person?

How can listening not be verbal? "The role of the listener in any verbal episode was thus "not necessarily verbal in any special sense.""
I think I understand the unworkability of this definition, I just want to understand Skinner's conceptualization.
Please don't tell me I have to read Verbal Behaviour.

I'm not quite understanding Skinner's dilemma regarding a functional definition of "specifying". He cannot refer to reference. I understand that, but am not getting the inability to refer to verbal behaviour. Is this why? "(Skinner)did not distinguish between verbal rules and regularities observed in other complex antecedents." p17

thanks
Joanne

jsteinwachs

Summary of Chapter One

Summary of Chapter One

Here is my summary of chapter one. I know i am a little behind schedule :(
but would be grateful if anyone could point out any fundamental misunderstandings before i get too far with ch 2. Unfortunately the formatting of headings has disappeared but i guess that doesn't make much difference to how much sense it makes.

Chapter 1

Why a behavioural approach is a good idea.

This chapter opens by (briefly) making a case for a behavioural approach to language. It points to the progress made the behavioural approach in other areas, and to the difficulty the cognitive approach has had in stepping outside common sense assumptions (e.g. that words can be understood as representing ideas). The cognitive approach is said to provide a reasonable account within its own assumptions (i.e. that the task is to describe how words and sentences etc. are perceived, encoded, produced etc.). However, a behavioural approach may have something different and useful to offer.

The chapter continues by saying that language is, or should be, of fundamental importance to behaviour analysts, pointing out that the early focus on the behaviour of non-verbal organisms was only ever intended to be a starting place.

What sort of behavioural approach we are talking about.

The authors outline the assumptions of their particular brand of behaviour analysis, i.e. functional contextualism. This is really very simple. If you start from the goals (predict and influence) everything else follows. These goals (predict and influence) mean that the independent variables must be in the environment of the person/system/group being studied (else, obviously, the theory cannot tell you how to change the person/group/system). Hence the importance of context or environment. Also, ‘truth’ has to be what works in achieving those goals. There is no other criterion. That’s all there is to it. (??)

Why the previous attempts of behaviourists (even Skinner’s) to provide an account of language didn’t work and why this one will be better.

The next few sections of the chapter are concerned with outlining previous attempts by people working within the behavioural tradition to develop accounts of human language. Since these appear to have come unstuck in some way or other, it is important to understand what went wrong, and perhaps to explain the ways in which the current approach is different and stands a better chance of success.

The first to be considered is Kantor’s interbehavioural approach, which apparently did not generate a viable programme of research. The case is made that this is because it was based on descriptive rather than functional contextualism. In other words, the lesson is look what happens if you do not have your goals in place. (?) Some valuable and influential aspects of his work are also outlined: specifically, the importance of context and the idea that the stimulus functions of an event should be seen as part of the response to the event and that stimulus functions can be transferred from one event to another.

The chapter then looks at Skinner’s approach to language, and two attempts (those of Willard Day and Kurt Salzinger) to obtain the empirical data that would be needed to test and develop this model. The main point of this section is to argue that Skinner’s work, although an enormous step forward at the time, is not up to the job of moving us forward now. Basically if it was going to work we’d be a lot further ahead by now. The authors say that if you are happy to accept this conclusion you can skip the next few pages, but if you need to be convinced then you have to read them. I guess people who are familiar with Skinner’s Verbal Behaviour would find this section easy to follow, but I’m not and I didn’t. However the main point seems to be that there is problem with his definition of verbal behaviour: you decide whether or not a piece of behaviour is verbal by seeing how it is reinforced. If it is reinforced by someone who has been trained to reinforce it in this way by the verbal community then the behaviour is verbal. However, the behaviour of the listener/reinforcer is not itself necessarily verbal (it could be reinforced in some other way). This is a problem, because trained ‘listeners’ who deliver the reinforcement could be reinforcing all sorts of different behaviours that we wouldn’t generally consider verbal, such as the behaviour of rats in operant conditioning experiments. This makes the definition both too broad (it includes rats in operant conditioning experiments) and too narrow (it excludes the behaviour of the listener).

The research has led to RFT

The last part of the chapter outlines the work in behaviour analysis that paved the way for RFT. There are two main strands of research.

The first to be considered is research on rule-governed behaviour. Basically, instead of having to learn contingencies by trial and error you can just be told them and that way you get them much quicker, but you tend to take longer to notice if they turn out not to be what you were told they were. This would seem to be something to do with language (the “language hypothesis”). However, an account of rule-governed behaviour as verbal behaviour could not be accommodated within Skinner’s approach because a) the function of a stimulus as a rule depends on the history of the listener rather than the speaker and Skinner’s definition of verbal behaviour concerned only the speaker, and b) he could only say a rule was a discriminative stimulus which specifies a contingency, but this is inadequate (because it does not distinguish rules from other types of discriminative stimuli) unless you can specify what is meant by ‘specify’ which he was not able to do (following his own rules).

The second is derived relational responding. This section starts by outlining a famous paper by Sidman (1971) in which he reports that a person (child?) with learning difficulties who had been taught to match spoken words to pictures and separately taught to match the same set of spoken words to printed words was then able to match pictures to the corresponding printed words without actually being trained to do this. [NB I think there is an error in book near the top of page 18 in the sentence that begins, “Sidman’s (1971) …”?]. Because the picture-written words relationships had not been trained they are called derived. The next paragraph summarises how equivalence relations were studied using matching to sample procedures. If, having learnt that selecting X from X, Y and Z is correct in the presence of A, A is then selected from an array from stimuli such as A, B and C in the presence of X, then this is said to show symmetry. Further, if having learnt that selecting X is correct in the presence of A, and selecting P is correct in the presence of X, when then presented with an array of say P, Q and R in the presence of A, you pick P, well then that shows transitivity.

This was exciting because it seems very like language (e.g. the relationships between words and things are symmetrical). It was also puzzling because this symmetry does not generally apply in other situations, and the example of approaching thicket in the presence of a lion, but not a lion in the presence of a thicket is given.

The chapter ends by claiming that taking this idea of derived relational responding further (in the next chapters) leads to a more adequate definition of verbal behaviour, allows rule-governed behaviour to be understood as verbal, and lays the ground for a ‘vibrant research agenda’.

Thanks,

Janet

Janet Wingrove

questions on 1.1.3 and 1.2.1

questions on 1.1.3 and 1.2.1

Okay, help. Can someone give me a real life example that will help me distinguish a mand from a tact?
Tact: A verbal operant in which a response (from the person emitting the operant or from the environment) of a given form is evoked.. by a particular object or event or property of an event of an oject or event."

There is an excellent summary of the core ideas of Verbal Behavior in Kohlenberg and Tsai's 1991 FAP Book. The whole of chapter 3 is about these issues and is very interesting as is the whole book. I copy a few lines from it since they tell it much better than I could :

A tact is defined as a verbal response that is under the precise control of discriminative stimuli, and that is reinforced by generalized secondary reinforcers. For example, if you are shown a red ball and asked, "What is this?" and you say "A red ball," you would be tacting because the form of your response ("red ball") is controlled by the object and is reinforced by a conditioned generalized reinforcer such as "uh-huh," "right" or "thank you," or any of hundreds of reactions that indicate you were understood. Notice that the contingency or reinforcer is borad and general, whereas the prior discriminative stimulus (Sd) is specific. The tact is thus brought about by the presence of a particular stimulus (e.g., a red ball) and an audience (the therapist or parent). Tacts, in this sense, are similar to the notion of labels or names ( p.54)

1.1.3 Skinner's Approach p9
Mand: A verbal operant in which the response, (whose? the person emitting the operant or the environment?) is reinforced by a characteristic consequence and is therefore under the functional control of relevant conditions of deprivation or aversive stimulation.

Stil from the FAP book : Mands are the speech involved in demands, commands, requests, and questions. A mand is behavior with the following characteristics: (1) it occurs because it was followed by a particular reinforcer, (2) its strength varies with the relevant deprivation or aversive stimulation, and (3) it appears under a very broad range of discriminative stimuli. Thus, if you were to say, "I would like some water" because you were thirsty, this would be a mand because it would be reinforced by a very specific reinforcer - someone hearing you and giving you water or showing you were to get some. Your "I want some water" response would not be reinforced by a generalized secondary reinforcer such as someone saying "That's right," or "Thanks for sharing that with me," or "I understand what you said". It's strength would also vary with how water deprived you were. Your mand for water can occur in almost any setting where you are thirsty and there is another person who can hear you. (p.56)

1.2.1 The definition of verbal behavior is not functional.p12

I'm not sure I understand this sentence in the last PP on the page
The behavior is not superstitious: the contingency is non arbitrary and is produced by the rat's behaviour.

Hope this is correct behavioralese : If you give arbitrary reinforcement to a pigeon, the frequency of the behavior closely preceding the reinforcement (it could be odd !) will rise. Thus, the probability that this particular behavior will occur short before the next instance of arbitrary reinforcement will rise, so it will be reinforced again and so on. As a final result, the pigeon will in the end emit this particular behavior with a high frequency, although the behavior in itself has no effect whatsoever on the reinforcing contingency. Which is not the case in the described example : As I understand it, by pressing the lever, the rat slightly shakes the feedbag and as a result, every five presses in average, a food pellet is jarred loose. This reinforcement is not arbitrary and is produced by the rat's behavior.

Here's another one on p13.
...Leighland cited Skinners ...theorising that the restricted contingencies required for abstraction ( a highly precise form of stimulus control) could only arise from an extensive history of social mediation.
The whole next paragraph just compounds the murk.

I find it difficult too. Maybe the important thing is to understand the critic made to Skinner's VB : Making appeal to the history of the listener in order to understand the behavior of the speaker is said to be a «conceptual error» making the design of fruitful experimental strategies extraordinarily difficult.

Okay enough for now. My mind is acting up.
Joanne

So does mine
Philippe


Below is a conversation regarding these questions:

RE: 1.2.1 The Definition of verbal behavior is not functioning. (Submitted by JT Blackledge on June 7, 2006 - 8:42pm.)

Hi Joanne & all--

This can be a tough one to convey and grasp, but I'll give it a shot anyway. The issue is this: Skinner (1957) defined verbal behavior as any behavior emitted by a speaker that is reinforced through the mediation of a listener trained by a verbal community to mediate such reinforcement. In other words, a given behavior is verbal if it is reinforced by someone else who has been specifically trained by a "verbal community" to reinforce such behavior in certain ways.

From this perspective, the lever-pressing behavior of every rat & pigeon that has gone through an operant conditioning experiment is verbal, because the reinforcement they receive (often in the form of a food pellet, for example) for pressing the lever depends on the mediation of the experimenter (specifically trained by a "verbal community" to mediate such reinforcement) who programs when the machine dispenses reinforcers. An odd (but true) statement, and one that makes you wonder "why define verbal behavior in such a way that it includes the behavior of apparently 'non-verbal' organisms?"

But that's not the biggest problem with the definition--Skinner's definition of VB actually violates one of the central tenets of behaviorism: What matters is the FUNCTION of a behavior, which refers to a reliable relationship between the emission of a response and the delivery of a reinforcer. From a functional perspective, it doesn't matter what the response looks like. My good friend Pete Linnerooth describes it this way: You can walk to the fridge to get a beer, run to the fridge to get a beer, or crawl to the fridge to get a beer. Either way you get a beer. The form of the behavior doesn't matter. What holds a variety of different-looking behaviors together in the same class is their ability to receive the same kind of reinforcement--and we're interested in changing classes of behavior, not just specific instances (e.g., stop someone from running to the fridge to get a beer, and they can still walk, crawl, or swing from the rafters to get one...). From a functional perspective, it also doesn't matter how the reinforcement is delivered (e.g., if it magically drops from the Heavens, is delivered by Sue or Joe, or results from more 'natural' contingencies). What matters is that, in a given context, there is a reliable and predictable relationship between behavior and subsequent reinforcement. Hence the two examples on p. 12. If a rat gets a food pellet after pressing a lever 5 times (on average), it doesn't matter if this result is mediated by an experimenter, or if it simply occurs due to a more 'natural' set of cirucmstances (i.e., a hole in the food bag that releases a pellet after the bag is jarred every five times or so by a lever). The lever-pressing behavior will be shaped up and maintained regardless of the 'source' of the reinforcement, as long as the contingencies are relatively stable. To ascribe importance to anything other than this basic functional issue in a definition of verbal behavior doesn't make sense from a behavioral perspective, and places Skinner's definition of VB in contrast to virtually every functional definition within behavior analysis. (That being said, I still dig Skinner for the brilliant contributions he made....).

Hope this helps.

Best,
JT

JT Blackledge
Lecturer
University of Wollongong
New South Wales, Australia


The functional nature of Tacts and Mands. (Submitted by Aidan Hart on June 9, 2006 - 5:23am.)

I’m still attempting to understand many of the concepts in the RFT book and Skinners definition of verbal behaviour. So far, my contact with Skinners system has been primarily within the chapter contained within Kohlenberg and Tsai (1991).

However, it seems to me that Skinners system, (and in particular Tacts and Mands as verbal response classes) is indeed a functional one. How one distinguishes a tact from a mand is not based on the content of the verbal statement but on the function it serves. Does this not make it functional?

For example, a client recently remarked to me that they found aspects of our sessions difficult (I can’t remember the exact phrase that they used and I suppose the exact phraseology is not important). Skinners system, as explained by Kohlenberg and Tsai, provides a framework in which I can I attempt to understand the function of the clients statement.

Is the client’s verbal behaviour a Tact? If so, in this case the clients verbal behaviour is under the discriminative control of me as a therapist, the questions I might be asking (and have asked) and possibly the setting events of the therapy session itself, the clients description may then be reinforced by my response (“Yes, these are difficult things to talk about”) If the client has a history of being under assertive or does not like to admit when they are struggling, this Tact may also serve as a CRB2 and my response to them may help reinforce and build their ‘self-tacting’ and ‘tacting’ to others behaviour.

However, I also considered that the clients verbal statement may have been a Mand, albeit a disguised one. In tacting that the sessions were sometimes difficult, the client may also have been Manding that they wished to talk about something else. The client may have wanted recognition or praise for their efforts. So even though the content remains the same, the distinction between the tact and the mand in this case surely lies in the function of the verbal statement itself which itself can not be understood independent of my (and others) history of responding to such statements.

I think that there is merit in Skinners position concerning appealing to the history of the listener in order to understand the behaviour of the speaker. I think potentially what Skinner may have been arguing for here is an ‘appeal to the contingencies’. In attempting to understand the function of any verbal interaction and in particular the function of the speakers verbal behaviour we must understand the context in which the verbal behaviour has occurred. The listener is part of this context. Understanding the function of the listener in that context will indeed require an understanding of the history of the listener. If the speaker’s behaviour is under the discriminative or consequential control of the listener and the listener’s response, then we need to understand the behaviour of the listener and their history of interacting with the speaker, if we are to understand the function of the speaker’s behaviour in the first place.

In the example above, distinguishing between a tact and mand and determining the function of the mand in terms or appetitive or aversive control will require understanding our history of interaction. This requires understanding my history of responding to such descriptions or request from clients or this client in particular. Therefore understanding the clients (speakers) behaviour is surely contingent upon a functional understanding of my (the listeners) behaviour.

Just some musings.

 

re: the functional nature of tacts and mands. (Submitted by JT Blackledge on June 10, 2006 - 8:24pm.)

Hi Aidan--

Very clever observation, me thinks. Tacts and mands may point to functional distinctions as they can describe responses made in the presence of different discriminative stimuli which function differently (e.g., a mand is a response reinforced by the acquisition of a tangible reinforcer; a tact reinforced by attention in the form of social praise). The problem is, use of these terms doesn't add anything functionally new to the analysis given Skinner's core definition of verbal behavior. In the bar-pressing examples used in the book, for example, the rat's bar-press on a reinforcement schedule controlled by the experimenter would be considered a mand; the rat's bar-press on a reinforcement schedule controlled by the 'hole in the feed bag' would simply be a response made in the presence of a discriminative stimulus (or SD) which functions to acquire a tangible reinforcer. But either way, the SD (in this case, say the bar or lever) functions exactly the same way (to elicit a bar-pressing response) and exactly the same schedule of reinforcement results. In other words, describing the rat's behavior as verbal or non-verbal adds nothing functionally useful to the analysis--the rat will continue to respond as generally expected for a rat receiving food pellets on a VI5 schedule, and no new terms based on the learning history of the listener or experimenter need be forwarded. In fact, by implying that the learning history of the listener/experimenter needs to be taken into account in order to determine 'what kind of behavior' the rat is engaging in, Skinner (perhaps inadvertantly) departed from a core focus in behavior analysis: It is the learning history of the individual under analysis, and how this history ascribes differential functions to current contexts, that is solely of interest. Plus, the way Skinner defined VB turned out not to address the differences in responding so far empirically observed between humans and non-humans (the fact that we can talk about a rat emitting verbal behavior using Skinner's definition points to part of the problem).

Stimulus equivalence theory and RFT manage to draw functionally useful distinctions between verbal and non-verbal behavior, distinctions that have repeatedly held up under empirical scrutiny. RFT in particular draws the dividing line between VB and non-VB solely at a process level (i.e., derived relational responding in the case of verbal behavior; operant behavior based on direct contingencies and generalization in the case of non-verbal behavior) that can be witnessed by focusing solely on the learning history of the individual under analysis.

And virtually all of this particular thread of the discussion is probably unimportant to those of you simply trying to understand RFT better. I think it only really becomes important when you are trying to sort through different behavioral accounts of verbal behavior and determine which one has a better smell to it.

Fun stuff, anyway.

Best,
JT


The role of the listener. (Submitted by Jacqueline A-Tjak on June 14, 2006 - 1:54am.)

Hi Aidan and all,
John and Francis are right: we do not need to understand Skinners view on VB or the corrections RFT made to understand the RFT stuff. Still there is some fun in it and I sure am glad with the questions asked, all the comments, examples and definitions that were given by others (on another childpage).
The role of the listener, Aidan, seems very important to me too. And he would not have his role as listener if he did not have a history of learning to respond/reinforce the words of the speaker.
But as I get it, focussing on the history of the listener brings about so many problems that verbal behavior cannot be studied in a way that helps us to understand human behavior better. John explained some of these problems. So RFT changed the definition of verbal behavior. This does not mean that studying the role of the listener suddenly does not seem important at all any more. It is just not a part of the definition any more.
Still, I do think you are very right that it is the interaction that 'needs' to be studied: behavior in context. The listener is (part of the) context for the speaker. The listeners behavior (which has a history) can be reinforcing for the speaker. The listeners behavior can now be seen as verbal behavior and the listener can now be seen as a speaker and the speaker as a listener, who reinforces the respons of the speaker who was a listener at first etc. Which is a complicated way to say that therapist and client mutually influence each others behavior.

Jacqueline

The role of the listener. (Submitted by Marco Kleen on June 14, 2006 - 4:16am.)

Hi everybody,

I think you hit an important point here, Jacqueline. The clinical implications of mutual influence of speakers and listeners are huge. This subject is very related to things like therapeutic relation, motivation, 'nonspecific variables' and so on. Just focussing on the intrapsychological processes would mean ignoring the role of the listener (and observer in experiments) as important contextual factor for the speaker.
A question: does that mean that the only thing a therapist can theoretically do is influencing the context because he's nothing more than a (relevant) contexual factor, or are there more theoretical possibilities?

Marco

The role of the listener. (Submitted by JT Blackledge on June 14, 2006 - 9:18pm.)

Hi Marco--

The issue you mention (the mutual influence of therapists and clients) is critically important--I wholeheartedly agree that we can and should monitor both therapist and client behavior, and that we should be interested in monitoring how therapist and client interact and in changing the behaviors of both parties for the better. And the function issue as it relates to verbal behavior is actually a different issue than that. The behavioral assumption is that stimuli take on different functions for an individual solely because of the learning history of the individual. When I'm interacting with a client in therapy, whatever I do functions in specific ways for the client solely because of the learning history of the client. Once we've started interacting, the client's interactions with me become part of his learning history--but it's still solely his learning history (intersecting with current context) that determines functions of stimuli in his environment, and thus determines his behavior. And certainly, many of the stimuli 'presented' to the client by me will be dependent on my learning history as the therapist. But the functions those stimuli take on for the client still depend entirely on the interaction between the client's learning history and the current context (not on ontological claim--just a pragmatic one). In other words, it's the client or speaker's learning history--not the therapist or listener's learning history--that determines what functions stimuli take on for the speaker from moment to moment.

The biggest take-home message from chapter 1, I think, is that it sets up the notion that RFT adds a clear description of how verbal behavior allows stimuli to take on different functions for an individual in a way (through derived relational responding) that's a bit different from non-verbal processes like direct contingency operant conditioning. That's one critically important thing RFT does that Skinner's analysis doesn't (Skinner assumed that verbal behavior and nonverbal behavior lead to stimuli taking on functions in the same ways). The second thing is that RFT does this without stating that we have to focus on the learning histories of both the client (e.g., the speaker) and the therapist (e.g., the listener) simultaneously in order to determine what function a given stimulus has for the client/speaker. It's the speaker's learning history that determines how a stimulus (even a stimulus provided by a "listener") functions for the speaker at any given moment--not the listener's learning history.

Anyway, fun to talk about.

Best,
JT


Mands and Tacts. (Submitted by JKesselring on June 7, 2006 - 4:20am.)

I'll have a go at the mand vs tact distinction. Consider this example: Bill and Joe are walking together down a street when they see a mutual acquantance ahead of them. Joe says to Bill, "What's that guys name?" and Bill replies "Jack". In this case the specific form of Bill's response (saying "Jack")was controlled by the specific properties of the stimulus (the guy calls himself Jack). This is a tact. [Joes question is a mand that sets the occasion for Bills response, but lets not worry about that now.]

Since Bill and Joe walk faster than Jack they gradually catch up to him. When they get a few feet behind him Bill says "Jack" which leads Jack to stop, turn toward them, and start to interact. In this situation a response with the same (or similar) formal characteristics as in the tact example (saying "Jack") has different functional characteristics. This is a mand since Bill said it "to get Jacks attention." Or to put it more behaviorally, he responded this way because in the past he has been reinforced by social interaction in similar contexts when he has said a person's name.

Another example: Joe says to his friend, Moe, who just baked a cake, "That cake looks tasty." This response would probably be a tact in the case where Joe has eaten a big meal and is throughly satiated. He is describing stimulus characteristics of the cake because in the past describing positive characteristics of the food that Moe prepares has been reinforced in various ways.

On the other hand, Joe's verbalization would be a mand in the case where he is food deprived and statements of this type in Moe's presence have been reinforced by food in the past. In this case Joe is says it because in similar contexts he has a history of being given a piece of the food after making positive remarks about the food.

Nontechnically speaking, the same words might be a compliment or a request for food (or both). (If his words did function as a request for food some would call them a "disguised mand" since they formally sound like a tact -- i.e., like a statement). The definition of a tact emphasizes the form of the response being controlled by of the properties of a contextual stimulus (in this case the cake or features of the cake), while the definition of the mand emphasizes the characteristics of the reinforcing stimulus relative to a deprivation condition (or other establishing operation).

I hope I didn't further confuse things.

John Kesselring


Mands. (Submitted by samtully on June 6, 2006 - 10:00pm.)

Correct me if I am wrong. I understand mands to be behavior, as outlined by Phillippe, though not limited to speech. i.e. if I wave at my friend to get him to come over this would be a mand. When a baby cries for food, this would be a mand. If a child spits to get attention, this is a mand. And of course, textual mands as well as sign language.

Cheers!
Shelly

jsteinwachs

Chapter 2

Chapter 2
This page is for summaries, discussions, and questions about Chapter 2 of the RFT book.
  • To add a new summary, question, or discussion item, click on the add a child page link at the bottom of this page. This will create a new web page for your summary or question. It will also create a link to your new page at the bottom of this page.
  • To respond to a summary, question, or discussion item someone has already posted, go to the page that contains their question or summary and click on the add new comment link at the bottom of that page. This will append your comment to the bottom of that page (it does not create a new web page). You can also reply to existing comments by clicking on the "reply" link in the comment.
Happy commenting, child paging, discussing, and learning!
Eric Fox

C func equation, page 33

C func equation, page 33

I have read and reread (and reread again!) chapter 2 over the past month, and find it much denser even than chapter 1. The concept I get hung up on over and over is the notion of arbitrary applicability (AA). While I know that AA is defined as a relationship that depends on social whim or consensus instead of formal characteristics of the stimulus, I think I’m missing the implications.

If we look at the equation for Cfunc as expressed on page 33, there’s a sentence that follows that says: “[w]e can say it this way: given arbitrarily applicable stimulus relations between A, B, and C, and given a context that actualizes the transformation of a given function of A, the functions of B and C will be modified in terms of the underlying relations between A, B, and C.”

I’d like to substitute examples for “A, B, and C”—could someone tell me if I’m on the right track with this?
An example of an actualizing context would be people talking to each other, having a conversation.
In that context, A might be a banana (the fruit itself, not the word).
One person could say, “Have you ever noticed how her nose [B, in my example?] looks like a banana [the oral word for the fruit, C in my example?]?”
Now, this comparison relies on both people’s previous experience with bananas. If the person responded “What’s a banana?” the first person would have to whip out a banana, or at least a picture of a banana, for purposes of comparison.
If this is right so far, one question would be, does one of the 3 items in the relation have to be something with material existence, not “just” a word? I think the answer to this is “no” because what if my characters were talking about something abstract?

So . . .
One person might say, “Love [A] is blind [B].”
Would C in this example be the quality of not being sighted, of blindness?
I’m getting tangled up here. Help!


Response Submitted

Funcky Cstuff. (Submitted by Jacqueline A-Tjak on August 1, 2006 - 2:55am.)

Hi Leslie,

I hope I do this right and this message will appear as an reply to your post.
The example you give of nose and banana seems right to me. The function: 'bended' tranfers/transforms from the actual banana to the nose (word).
Now the second example. Here is a 'simple' way to look at this: I do not know whether you need the third part (C). Transfer of function can happen too when it is about two relata, A en B, Love and blindness. There would be a transfer of the function: not seeing properly from the word blind to the word love.

Now lets make it more complicated:
The word blind refers to actually being blind or seeing someone that is blind (modeling). You could understand the sentence 'love is blind' without ever having seen that, just by hearing people tell about it.
With 'love' its even more complicated, because that is an even more complex experience.

Your question was: does one of the 3 items in the relation have to be something with material existence, not “just” a word.
The answer, as far as I understand it, is no, but....Somewhere in the learning history has to be direct experience. If you take the example of numbers: They are abstract, but we learn them by counting actual things. So your example would not be about three relata, but maybe thirty relata that add up to 'love is blind'. And one or more of these relata would be derived from experience.
And I think it would be far to complicated to try and figure out what relata you need to get at 'love is blind'.
So for the sake of understanding the matter maybe it is easier and enough to stay with the two relata.

Well, I hope this makes sense and it is a good test to see if I get what RFTstuff is about.

Jacqueline

Leslie Telfer

Pg 30 mutual entailment and transformation / transfer of stimulus function

Pg 30 mutual entailment and transformation / transfer of stimulus function

On page 30, first full paragraph, there is a description of a natural language event--someone names a ball to a child. It's being used to illustrate mutual entailment. But it seems that the last sentence "the r response in other words, will involve responding to the sound "ball" in terms of the previously experienced functions of actual balls." So that seems to be a description of the transfer of stimulus functions.

Is transfer of stimulus function a more precise term than transformation of stimulus function. Is transfer of stimulus function a subset of transformation of stimulus function that applies with frames of coordination or is it a different thing altogether?

Joanne Steinwachs

Leslie Telfer


Response to initial post-

Just go on reading... (Submitted by Philippe Vuille on August 5, 2006 - 9:28am.)

The response to your question is to be found on pages 31-32 :

«Equivalence research has repeatedly revealed that stimulus functions commonly transfer through the members of equivalence classes.»

Consider the classical example given by Catania (Learning, 4th ed. p. 154) :

«A child has learned to obey a parent's words, go and stop, when crossing with the parent at a traffic intersection. In a separate setting, the child is taught that go and green traffic lights are equivalent and that stop and red traffic lights are equivalent (in other words, go and green become members of one equivalence class and stop and red become members of another). If the discriminative functions of the words go and stop transfer to the respective traffic lights, the child will obey the traffic lights without any additional instruction.»

RFT is not only about equivalence or "coordination frames". In the case of frames such as opposition, the stimulus functions will not be merely transferred but transformed, as is shown in the example on page 32 :

«(...) Suppose a person is trained to selext stimulus B as the "opposite" of stimulus A. Now suppose that A is given a conditioned punishing function, such as by pairing it with a loss of points. It might be predicted that B would then have reinforcing functions (without having that function directly trained), by virtue of its "opposite" relation to the punishing A stimulus (...) It hardly seems right to say that the reinforcing effects "transferred" in such a case, because they were acquired indirectly through the relation of opposition between B and a punisher. It seems more proper to use the term transformation than transfer, and it is for this reason that RFT has adopted transformation of stimulus functions as the general term for this effect. We will still use the term transfer of stimulus functions, but will generally reserve it for situations in which the underlying relation leads to derived functions that are similar to those that were trained or that pre-existed.»

Philippe

jsteinwachs

Some questions

Some questions

Hi all,

Here are some questions I have after reading chapter 2.

1. In paragraph 2.1.1 (page 22/24) is explained what overarching purely functional operants are. Could one say that 'avoidance' is an overarching purely functional operant?

2. Does anyone know what 'self-discrimination functions' are? They are mentioned on page 32 in a paragraph about tranformation of stimulusfunctions (2.2.3)as an example of stimulusfunctions that have been shown to transfer.
As a whole, I still find the concept of stimulusfunctions very difficult to grasp. What I find especially difficult is to find good examples and to specify stimulusfunctions that are involved when looking at a real life example. Anyone who can help out here with examples?

3. In paragraph 2.4 families of relational frames are summed up (page 35-39). If I get it right the phrase: 'snakes are dangerous' means NOT that the snakes are in a relation of coordination, which should be understood as equivalence, but in a relation of hierarchie, like the phrase: John is an man. Snakes are a part of dangerous stuff. Is this right?

4. Am I right that stimulusfunctions can be relata?

Jacqueline


A discussion of this chapter is included below:

re: Some questions. (Submitted by JT Blackledge on September 4, 2006 - 11:02pm.)

Hi Jacqueline--

Good questions--I've inserted some answers below.

1. In paragraph 2.1.1 (page 22/24) is explained what overarching purely functional operants are. Could one say that 'avoidance' is an overarching purely functional operant?

I think it would be slightly more technically correct to term avoidance behaviors as members of the same functional class--as avoidance behaviors have over the relatively long history of behavior analysis. Reserving the word 'overarching' for language (for example) allows the term to illustrate how operants can be grouped together even if these operants involve different functions.
(this answer might make more sense after reading the answer to 2, below).

2. Does anyone know what 'self-discrimination functions' are? They are mentioned on page 32 in a paragraph about tranformation of stimulusfunctions (2.2.3)as an example of stimulusfunctions that have been shown to transfer.
As a whole, I still find the concept of stimulusfunctions very difficult to grasp. What I find especially difficult is to find good examples and to specify stimulusfunctions that are involved when looking at a real life example. Anyone who can help out here with examples?

A stimulus function essentially refers to three things: the stimulus itself, the response made with respect to it, and the [punishing or reinforcing] consequence received for making that response. For example, you could say that a stimulus (e.g., a snake) serves an avoidance function if the presentation of a snake leads a subject to run away, and this 'running away' response is negatively reinforced by the 'removal' of the snake. Or, you could say that a child screaming serves an attention function if the screaming--made in response to an adult previously ignoring him--results in the adult positively reinforcing the child by attending to him. Finally, a 'self-discrimination function' might be evident if the verbal stimulus "Where are you, Jacqueline?" resulted in you responding "I'm right here", which in turn resulted in me positively reinforcing you. Essentially then, a "stimulus function" refers to a stimulus, a response made to that stimulus, and a consequence for that response. The 'whole behavioral unit', so to speak.

I've noticed the term "stimulus function" used more loosely & informally. For example, one might say that film of a combat scene serves an "emotive function", which would imply that the stimulus (the film) makes the viewer feel a certain way. Or, the term 'experiential avoidance' might be used to describe an attempt to avoid an unpleasant feeling that doesn't 'succeed' (i.e., doesn't yield negative reinforcment by immediate termination of the unpleasant feeling). Not entirely technically correct usage, as instances like this don't specify the consequence to the response-- though it does describe, in varying detail, the topographical nature of the response.

3. In paragraph 2.4 families of relational frames are summed up (page 35-39). If I get it right the phrase: 'snakes are dangerous' means NOT that the snakes are in a relation of coordination, which should be understood as equivalence, but in a relation of hierarchie, like the phrase: John is an man. Snakes are a part of dangerous stuff. Is this right?

Correct. The critical thing to think about when deciding if two stimuli are in a hierarchical relation vs. a relation of coordination is this: Does 'reversing' the relation make sense? If it's a coordinative relation, it will make logical sense without changing the relational term (e.g., changing "noir is black" to "black is noir" makes sense, as does changing "pictures are snapshots" to "snapshots are pictures"). If it's a hierarchical relation, it won't make sense: changing either "snakes are dangerous" to "dangerous are snakes" or "dogs are mammals" to "mammals are dogs" doesn't work, since in each case, one of the relata are members of a larger class and do not exclusively or exhaustively define the class.

4. Am I right that stimulusfunctions can be relata?

I think they would be more technically referred to as stimuli, but this relates to your question #2. Within behavior analysis, a "stimulus function" refers to a stimulus, the response made with respect to that stimulus, and the consequence that follows. If you think of relata as stimuli, and grant that (within behavior analysis) stimuli are typically only talked about if they have a function (why include a stimulus in a functional analysis unless it affects subsequent behavior?), then it follows that, for practical purposes, every stimulus has a function--and that any relata included in a meaningful functional analysis are thus not only stimuli, but also partipate in stimulus functions.

Best,
JT

JT Blackledge
Lecturer
University of Wollongong
New South Wales, Australia

 

(Inevitably) some more questions. (Submitted by Jacqueline A-Tjak on September 5, 2006 - 7:09am.)

Hi JT

Thank you do much. Your answers made things more clear to me. And led to more questions.
In chapter one it is metioned that 'stimulusfunctions could be substitutive, no longer requiring the presence of a stimulusobject' (page 8. This is why I thought stimulusfunctions can be relata or stimuli on their own right. After reading your postand thinking it over I now come to the conclusion that there must be some kind of stimulus, non-verbal or verbal that 'contains' the stimulusfuntion and stimulusfunctions do not exist in a void. Do I understand this properly this way?

And do you mean to say that self-discrimination functions mean that a person discriminates himself in a way?

Now, you put stimulusfunctions in a frame of operant conditioning, if I may put it this way. But on page 31 the RFT book says that elicited conditioned emotional responses have been proven to be stimulusfunctions that transfer. Do you think/know, does this mean that these responses must be understood in a operant context, for instance that they are functioning as a discriminative stimulus? Or do stimulusfunctions also operate in a classical conditioning way?
You write:
I've noticed the term "stimulus function" used more loosely & informally. For example, one might say that film of a combat scene serves an "emotive function", which would imply that the stimulus (the film) makes the viewer feel a certain way. Or, the term 'experiential avoidance' might be used to describe an attempt to avoid an unpleasant feeling that doesn't 'succeed' (i.e., doesn't yield negative reinforcment by immediate termination of the unpleasant feeling). Not entirely technically correct usage, as instances like this don't specify the consequence to the response-- though it does describe, in varying detail, the topographical nature of the response.

Now what you write makes me think we should not view stimulusfunctions as something that happens in a classical conditioning situation, since there are no consequences in purely classical conditioning. I am not sure if you can, in ordinary life (as opposed to the laboratory) divide operant and classical conditioning, but I would like to make sure that I understand it correctly, if that is possible.

Lastly, you answered me that avoidance should be seen as a functional class, not an overarching operant. I am afraid that I do not understand why it is the first AND not the latter.

Thanks for having taken the trouble to clear things up for me and I hope you or somebody else can help me out with these new questions.

Best wishes

Jacqueline

 

RE: additional questions. (Submitted by JT Blackledge on September 5, 2006 - 9:14pm.)

Hi Jacqueline--
I've inserted answers below in ALL CAPS.

In chapter one it is metioned that 'stimulusfunctions could be substitutive, no longer requiring the presence of a stimulusobject' (page 8. This is why I thought stimulusfunctions can be relata or stimuli on their own right. After reading your postand thinking it over I now come to the conclusion that there must be some kind of stimulus, non-verbal or verbal that 'contains' the stimulusfuntion and stimulusfunctions do not exist in a void. Do I understand this properly this way?

YES--I CAN DEFINITELY SEE HOW THAT SENTENCE IS MISLEADING. THE 'UNWRITTEN SUBTEXT' THERE IS THAT THE STIMULUS IS NOW NOT AN ACTUAL LIGHT, BUT A VERBAL STIMULUS LIKE "IMAGINE A LAMP SITTING IN FRONT OF YOU". THE SENTENCE MIGHT MORE ACCURATELY READ "STIMULUS FUNCTIONS COULD BE SUBSTITUTIVE, NO LONGER REQUIRING THE PRESENCE OF THE ORIGINAL STIMULUS OBJECT".

And do you mean to say that self-discrimination functions mean that a person discriminates himself in a way?

YES

Now, you put stimulusfunctions in a frame of operant conditioning, if I may put it this way. But on page 31 the RFT book says that elicited conditioned emotional responses have been proven to be stimulusfunctions that transfer. Do you think/know, does this mean that these responses must be understood in a operant context, for instance that they are functioning as a discriminative stimulus? Or do stimulusfunctions also operate in a classical conditioning way?

You write:
I've noticed the term "stimulus function" used more loosely & informally. For example, one might say that film of a combat scene serves an "emotive function", which would imply that the stimulus (the film) makes the viewer feel a certain way. Or, the term 'experiential avoidance' might be used to describe an attempt to avoid an unpleasant feeling that doesn't 'succeed' (i.e., doesn't yield negative reinforcment by immediate termination of the unpleasant feeling). Not entirely technically correct usage, as instances like this don't specify the consequence to the response-- though it does describe, in varying detail, the topographical nature of the response.

Now what you write makes me think we should not view stimulusfunctions as something that happens in a classical conditioning situation, since there are no consequences in purely classical conditioning. I am not sure if you can, in ordinary life (as opposed to the laboratory) divide operant and classical conditioning, but I would like to make sure that I understand it correctly, if that is possible.

VERY NICE CATCH OF A VERY FINE-GRAINED POINT. THINK OF IT THIS WAY: A GIVEN STIMULUS CAN TAKE ON DIFFERENT FUNCTIONS DUE TO VARIOUS PROCESSES--CLASSICAL CONDITIONING, OPERANT CONDITIONING, STIMULUS GENERALIZATION, DERIVED RELATIONAL RESPONDING. ONCE A STIMULUS HAS A FUNCTION, REGARDLESS OF THE PROCESS(ES) INVOLVED IN TAKING ON THAT FUNCTION, THAT FUNCTION STILL TECHNICALLY REFERS TO THAT STIMULUS, THE RESPONSE THAT IS MADE TO THAT STIMULUS, AND THE CONSEQUENCE THAT FOLLOWS.

YOU'RE DEFINITELY RIGHT ABOUT THE ARBITRARY DISTINCTION BETWEEN CLASSICAL AND OPERANT CONDITIONING. INDEED, IT'S BEEN ARGUED THAT AN OPERANT CAN BE DESCRIBED IN PURELY RESPONDENT TERMS (I.E., THE DISCRIMINATIVE STIMULUS IS CLASSICALLY CONDITIONED TO THE RESPONSE, WHICH IN TURN IS CLASSICALLY CONDITIONED TO THE CONSEQUENCE). IT'S JUST THE PRACTICAL UTILITY IN DISTINGUISHING BETWEEN TWO PROCESSES THAT KEEPS THEM SEPARATE, METHINKS.

Lastly, you answered me that avoidance should be seen as a functional class, not an overarching operant. I am afraid that I do not understand why it is the first AND not the latter.

IN APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS, IT'S COMMMON TO TALK ABOUT BEHAVIOR HAVING ONE OF FOUR FUNCTIONS: AVOIDANCE (OF AVERSIVE STIMULATION), ACQUISTION (OF TANGIBLE REINFORCERS), ATTENTION (I.E., BEHAVIOR THAT FUNCTIONS TO RECEIVE POSITIVE REINFORCEMENT IN THE FORM OF ATTENTION), AND SELF-STIMULATION. ANY BEHAVIOR, REGARDLESS OF HOW DIFFERENT THOSE BEHAVIORS LOOK, THAT SERVES ONE OF THOSE FUNCTIONS (E.,G., AN AVOIDANCE FUNCTION) FALLS INTO THAT RESPECTIVE FUNCTIONAL CATEGORY. IN OTHER WORDS, BEHAVIORS ARE GROUPED INTO FUNCTIONAL CLASSES SOLELY ACCORDING TO FUNCTION--A FUNCTIONAL CLASS, BY DEFINITION, INCLUDES A VARIETY OF STIMULUS/RESPONSES THAT ALL SERVE PRECISELY THE SAME FUNCTION.

OVERARCHING OPERANTS--LIKE MUTUAL/COMBINATIORIAL ENTAILMENT--INVOLVE PROCESSES THAT OPERATE REGARDLESS OF WHAT SUBSEQUENT FUNCTIONS RESULT. IN ANY GIVEN CASE OF COMBINATORIAL ENTAILMENT, FOR EXAMPLE, THE RESULTING FUNCTION MIGHT BE AVOIDANCE, ACQUISITION, ATTENTION, OR SELF-STIMULATION. THINK OF AN OVERARCHING OPERANT AS A PROCESS THAT ALLOWS VARIOUS FUNCTIONS TO 'ATTACH' THEMSELVES (SO TO SPEAK) TO STIMULI IN A CLEARLY SPECIFIED WAY--AND FUNCTIONAL CLASSES AS GROUPINGS OF STIMULUS/RESPONSES THAT SHARE THE SAME FUNCTION (REGARDLESS OF THE PROCESSES THROUGH WHICH THESE FUNCTIONS AROSE).

Thanks for having taken the trouble to clear things up for me and I hope you or somebody else can help me out with these new questions.

NO PROBLEM--IT STRETCHES MY BRAIN :)

Best wishes

Jacqueline

JT Blackledge
Lecturer
University of Wollongong
New South Wales, Australia

 

One addition. (Submitted by JT Blackledge on September 6, 2006 - 1:11am.)

Actually, you know what, the more I think about it, the more I recall that it would be correct to talk about a respondent function as including only the classically conditioned stimulus and the response made with respect to it (theoretically speaking, no consequence). That is, indeed, the 'whole behavioral unit', when speaking about a respondent, so why not?

JT Blackledge
Lecturer
University of Wollongong
New South Wales, Australia

 

More brainstretching exercises. (Submitted by Jacqueline A-Tjak on September 6, 2006 - 3:22am.)

Hi JT,

Thanks for clearing things for me and in the proces of doing so make me feel that I am asking questions that are worth answering. Asking these kind of questions always make me feel awkward, since my mind produces a lot of chatter in the proces.

Now if I understand everything you have written, it follows that the phrase: "one might say that film of a combat scene serves an "emotive function", which would imply that the stimulus (the film) makes the viewer feel a certain way (I quoted you here) is actually in accordance with the way stimulusfunctions work. What the speakers means/could mean is that the movie by classical conditioning or derivation has acquired a conditional emotional responsfunction. Is this correct?

Now here comes a difficult question for me to ask:
You wrote a very fine article about RFT: Blacklegde J.T. (2003) An Introduction to Relational Frame Theory: Basics and Applications. (The Behavior Analyst Today, 3, 4, 421-433). I recommend it to everyone who wants to know more about RFT. Soon I will be giving a course in ACT and tell something about RFT and your article is among the literature the coursemembers have to read.
AND..... You say in this article that Snakes and Danger are in a relation of coordinance. So, would this mean you made a mistake there?

Kind regards,

Jacqueline

 

re: stretching. (Submitted by JT Blackledge on September 6, 2006 - 6:22pm.)

Yes indeed--danger would stand in a hierarchical relation to snakes (not a coordinative one), wouldn't it?

JT Blackledge
Lecturer
University of Wollongong
New South Wales, Australia

 

Stimulus Functions. (Submitted by Jacqueline A-Tjak on September 17, 2006 - 9:21am.)

Hi all,

Still strugling with the concept of stimulusfunctions (the RFT book does not give a proper definition, does it?) I would like to know if one could say the following to be technically correct:
"Stimulusfunctions specify the nature of influence (not the intensity of it) of a certain stimulus to a certain respons". For instance, a stimulus could have a discriminative function, which would mean that it signals to a person that certain behavior has a acceptable probability to be reinforced. Since we are talking about probabilities you cannot say the stimulusfunction specifies the influence, only the nature of the influence (we are never sure beforehand this influence actually will show itself).

Jacqueline

Jacqueline A-Tjak

Chapter 3

Chapter 3
This page is for summaries, discussions, and questions about Chapter 3 of the RFT book.
  • To add a new summary, question, or discussion item, click on the add a child page link at the bottom of this page. This will create a new web page for your summary or question. It will also create a link to your new page at the bottom of this page.
  • To respond to a summary, question, or discussion item someone has already posted, go to the page that contains their question or summary and click on the add new comment link at the bottom of that page. This will append your comment to the bottom of that page (it does not create a new web page). You can also reply to existing comments by clicking on the "reply" link in the comment.
Happy commenting, child paging, discussing, and learning!
Eric Fox

Chapter 4

Chapter 4
This page is for summaries, discussions, and questions about Chapter 4 of the RFT book.
  • To add a new summary, question, or discussion item, click on the add a child page link at the bottom of this page. This will create a new web page for your summary or question. It will also create a link to your new page at the bottom of this page.
  • To respond to a summary, question, or discussion item someone has already posted, go to the page that contains their question or summary and click on the add new comment link at the bottom of that page. This will append your comment to the bottom of that page (it does not create a new web page). You can also reply to existing comments by clicking on the "reply" link in the comment.
Happy commenting, child paging, discussing, and learning!
Eric Fox

Chapter 5

Chapter 5
This page is for summaries, discussions, and questions about Chapter 5 of the RFT book.
  • To add a new summary, question, or discussion item, click on the add a child page link at the bottom of this page. This will create a new web page for your summary or question. It will also create a link to your new page at the bottom of this page.
  • To respond to a summary, question, or discussion item someone has already posted, go to the page that contains their question or summary and click on the add new comment link at the bottom of that page. This will append your comment to the bottom of that page (it does not create a new web page). You can also reply to existing comments by clicking on the "reply" link in the comment.
Happy commenting, child paging, discussing, and learning!
Eric Fox

Chapter 6

Chapter 6
This page is for summaries, discussions, and questions about Chapter 6 of the RFT book.
  • To add a new summary, question, or discussion item, click on the add a child page link at the bottom of this page. This will create a new web page for your summary or question. It will also create a link to your new page at the bottom of this page.
  • To respond to a summary, question, or discussion item someone has already posted, go to the page that contains their question or summary and click on the add new comment link at the bottom of that page. This will append your comment to the bottom of that page (it does not create a new web page). You can also reply to existing comments by clicking on the "reply" link in the comment.
Happy commenting, child paging, discussing, and learning!
Eric Fox

Chapter 7

Chapter 7
This page is for summaries, discussions, and questions about Chapter 7 of the RFT book.
  • To add a new summary, question, or discussion item, click on the add a child page link at the bottom of this page. This will create a new web page for your summary or question. It will also create a link to your new page at the bottom of this page.
  • To respond to a summary, question, or discussion item someone has already posted, go to the page that contains their question or summary and click on the add new comment link at the bottom of that page. This will append your comment to the bottom of that page (it does not create a new web page). You can also reply to existing comments by clicking on the "reply" link in the comment.
Happy commenting, child paging, discussing, and learning!
Eric Fox

Chapter 8

Chapter 8
This page is for summaries, discussions, and questions about Chapter 8 of the RFT book.
  • To add a new summary, question, or discussion item, click on the add a child page link at the bottom of this page. This will create a new web page for your summary or question. It will also create a link to your new page at the bottom of this page.
  • To respond to a summary, question, or discussion item someone has already posted, go to the page that contains their question or summary and click on the add new comment link at the bottom of that page. This will append your comment to the bottom of that page (it does not create a new web page). You can also reply to existing comments by clicking on the "reply" link in the comment.
Happy commenting, child paging, discussing, and learning!
Eric Fox

Chapter 9

Chapter 9
This page is for summaries, discussions, and questions about Chapter 9 of the RFT book.
  • To add a new summary, question, or discussion item, click on the add a child page link at the bottom of this page. This will create a new web page for your summary or question. It will also create a link to your new page at the bottom of this page.
  • To respond to a summary, question, or discussion item someone has already posted, go to the page that contains their question or summary and click on the add new comment link at the bottom of that page. This will append your comment to the bottom of that page (it does not create a new web page). You can also reply to existing comments by clicking on the "reply" link in the comment.
Happy commenting, child paging, discussing, and learning!
Eric Fox

Chapter 10

Chapter 10
This page is for summaries, discussions, and questions about Chapter 10 of the RFT book.
  • To add a new summary, question, or discussion item, click on the add a child page link at the bottom of this page. This will create a new web page for your summary or question. It will also create a link to your new page at the bottom of this page.
  • To respond to a summary, question, or discussion item someone has already posted, go to the page that contains their question or summary and click on the add new comment link at the bottom of that page. This will append your comment to the bottom of that page (it does not create a new web page). You can also reply to existing comments by clicking on the "reply" link in the comment.
Happy commenting, child paging, discussing, and learning!
Eric Fox

Chapter 11

Chapter 11
This page is for summaries, discussions, and questions about Chapter 11 of the RFT book.
  • To add a new summary, question, or discussion item, click on the add a child page link at the bottom of this page. This will create a new web page for your summary or question. It will also create a link to your new page at the bottom of this page.
  • To respond to a summary, question, or discussion item someone has already posted, go to the page that contains their question or summary and click on the add new comment link at the bottom of that page. This will append your comment to the bottom of that page (it does not create a new web page). You can also reply to existing comments by clicking on the "reply" link in the comment.
Happy commenting, child paging, discussing, and learning!
Eric Fox

Chapter 12

Chapter 12
This page is for summaries, discussions, and questions about Chapter 12 of the RFT book.
  • To add a new summary, question, or discussion item, click on the add a child page link at the bottom of this page. This will create a new web page for your summary or question. It will also create a link to your new page at the bottom of this page.
  • To respond to a summary, question, or discussion item someone has already posted, go to the page that contains their question or summary and click on the add new comment link at the bottom of that page. This will append your comment to the bottom of that page (it does not create a new web page). You can also reply to existing comments by clicking on the "reply" link in the comment.
Happy commenting, child paging, discussing, and learning!
Eric Fox

Chapter 13

Chapter 13
This page is for summaries, discussions, and questions about Chapter 13 of the RFT book.
  • To add a new summary, question, or discussion item, click on the add a child page link at the bottom of this page. This will create a new web page for your summary or question. It will also create a link to your new page at the bottom of this page.
  • To respond to a summary, question, or discussion item someone has already posted, go to the page that contains their question or summary and click on the add new comment link at the bottom of that page. This will append your comment to the bottom of that page (it does not create a new web page). You can also reply to existing comments by clicking on the "reply" link in the comment.
Happy commenting, child paging, discussing, and learning!
Eric Fox

Other Helpful Readings

Other Helpful Readings

Hi all,
I suggest that we use this page to post other readings so people don't have to dig through posts to find them. If it's not too much trouble, when you come across something, please post it here as well as in your original post. I'm thinking that all that we post here will be read by newcomers to ACT/RFT and I'd like to make it as easy as possible for them. It would also be useful if you'd give a few sentences on why you found it useful and what you found it useful for.

So far we've got:
Kohlenberg and Tsai (1991)

Blackledge, J. T. (2003). An introduction to Relational Frame Theory: Basics and applications. The Behavior Analyst Today, 3(4), 421-433.

Hayes, Get Out of Your Mind and Into Your Life

Hayes et al, Practical Guide to ACT

The RFT tutorial

Ciarrochi, J., Robb, H., & Godsell, C. (2005). Letting a little nonverbal air into the room: Insights from Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Part 1: Philosophical and theoretical underpinnings. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 23, 79-106.

Any others?

Thanks,
Joanne

jsteinwachs

Learning BA to understand RFT

Learning BA to understand RFT

Hi all,

For those of us who aren't behaviourally trained, I've been working through this Resources in Behavior Analysis page from the Cambridge Center.

It's wonderful:

https://behavior.org/

It's on the website, but I thought I'd put it here. The Skinner program on BA is helping me a lot with basic terminology.

Joanne Steinwachs

jsteinwachs

Academic Training

Academic Training

A number of academic training programs provide some measure of training in RFT. Go to Research Labs and click on a program or school's name to learn more about it.

ACBS Members: If you are a faculty member in an academic program that provides training in RFT, you can list your program clicking on the "research labs" link above and then "add child page" at the bottom of that page.
Eric Fox

RFT Email Listserv

RFT Email Listserv

The RFT email listserv is a forum for scientists, scholars, practitioners, and students to discuss Relational Frame Theory (RFT), an explicitly psychological account of human language and cognition. RFT is an approach designed to be a pragmatically useful analysis of complex human behavior, providing empirical and conceptual tools to conduct an experimental analysis of substantive topics in this arena. It is based on the principles of behavior analysis and contextual behavioral science. RFT is looked to as the conceptual basis for Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, as well as an increasingly large array of other applied interventions such as language training program, or programs designed to develop a sense of self in developmentally delayed children.

The RFT listserv is restricted to professionals or graduate students in relevant fields. Paid ACBS membership (student or professional) is required to join the RFT Listserv and you must be logged into your ACBS account.

Click here to join the RFT listserv

Community

RFT FAQ

RFT FAQ

 

Click on a question below to view its answer.

Eric Fox

How does Relational Frame Theory (RFT) relate to traditional CBT-theories?

How does Relational Frame Theory (RFT) relate to traditional CBT-theories?

That question is a huge one. RFT seeks a broad understanding of cognition. In the long run it could be more important than ACT because if it works the whole of psychology could change.

RFT is developmental, contextual, and behavioral. It gives you ideas about what to change to make things happen. It is so basic that it goes all the way down to animal behavior and human infants; and yet so broad in scope that it has clear implications for our understanding of social processes or such human activities as religion.

We have never had an empirically adequate behavioral, contextual account of cognition. Now we have at least the beginnings of one and it seems to be braking down the artificial barriers between cognitive and behavioral science.

The theories underlying CBT and CT are not like that. They have relatively low scope and they emerged typically from clinical concerns. They do not pretend to be the functional equivalent in cognition for what “behavioral principles” are in non-verbal behavior.

You have to be impressed with what the traditional behavior therapists were able to do with traditional behavioral principles, in part because these principles emphasized manipulable contextual variables. Imagine what we might do with a theory of cognition that emphasized manipulable contextual variables, if the theory was relatively adequate. Maybe a lot.

Steven Hayes

How is RFT different from stimulus equivalence?

How is RFT different from stimulus equivalence?

I often hear behavior analysts comment that RFT is essentially the same thing as stimulus equivalence, or that RFT does not add anything new to the equivalence literature.

Here are a few points to help clarify the difference between RFT and stimulus equivalence:

1) Stimulus equivalence is an empirical phenomenon; RFT is a behavioral theory about how that phenomenon (and other phenomena) comes about. In other words, RFT provides an operant analysis of how/why people are able to form equivalence classes. I cringe when I hear behavior analysts try to use stimulus equivalence as an explanation for some other behavioral event without recognizing that equivalence itself is a behavioral event that requires a technical analysis! This is especially true because the phenomenon of equivalence doesn't readily make sense from a direct contingency analysis (i.e., the "derived" or "emergent" relations are interesting because they're somewhat unexpected based on the organism's history of reinforcement in the presence of the "equivalent" stimuli...that is why we call the relations "derived" or "emergent").

2) Murray Sidman, a pioneer in behavior analysis and stimulus equivalence research, provided some of the earliest behavioral accounts of stimulus equivalence. Sidman's approach, however, was/is primarily a descriptive one: "My own theorizing has been directed not so much at an explanation of equivalence relations but rather, at the formulation of a descriptive system -- a consistent, coherent, and parsimonious way of defining and talking about the observed phenomena" (Sidman, 1994). A precise, coherent description of an empirical phenomenon is important, but it is not the same as a functional, behavioral explanation. RFT attempts to offer such an explanation.

3) In my view, RFT is a more scientifically conservative way to account for equivalence (as compared to Sidman's approach) because it does not require any new behavioral principles at the level of process (in Chapter 2 of the RFT book, we do propose a new behavioral principle at the level of outcome, but that is simply due to the unusual effects we see from the transformation of stimulus functions). Sidman, on the other hand, suggests that equivalence is probably a basic stimulus function "not derivable from more fundamental processes." RFT claims that it IS derivable from more fundamental processes -- basically, a history of multiple-exemplar training and differential reinforcement for relational responding.

4) The terms RFT uses to describe equivalence and other derived stimulus relations (e.g., mutual entailment, combinatorial entailment, transformation of stimulus functions) are more general than the terms used by Sidman (e.g., reflexivity, symmetry, transitivity, etc.). RFT introduced these new terms so that we could have a language to talk about all different types of stimulus relations (bigger than, before-after, opposites, darker than, etc.). Sidman's terms were taken from mathematical set theory and apply to equivalence relations, but do not work for other types of stimulus relations. In other words, RFT folks did not create new terms just for the hell of it or just because they sounded kewl.

5) RFT, as suggested in point 4, also emphasizes research on types of derived stimulus relations other than equivalence. This is important because stimulus functions can be changed on the basis of these derived relations, and the functions will be changed differently based on different relations. It has been demonstrated many times now that stimulus functions can transfer between members of an equivalence class (i.e., if one stimulus becomes a conditioned reinforcer, other stimuli in the equivalence class may also become reinforcers). But it has also been demonstrated that a stimulus can acquire new functions on the basis of a derived relation other than equivalence to another stimulus. For example, if stimulus A is a conditioned reinforcer and the learner derives a relation of opposition between stimulus A and B, stimulus B is likely to acquire aversive functions (the stimulus functions are transformed on the basis of its derived relation to stimulus A). This has been demonstrated with quite a few different types of stimulus functions and with quite a few different types of relations. Many researchers studying derived relations focus on equivalence relations exclusively, but RFT suggests knowledge of these other types of relations and their impact on the psychological functions of stimuli is also vitally important to a comprehensive theory of language and cognition.

Eric Fox

Free Videos: Learning About and Applying RFT

Free Videos: Learning About and Applying RFT

Here are some free videos that can help you learn about RFT.  These videos were not created for or by ACBS.  We merely gathered the videos from various sources for your convenience. (There are additional RFT-relevant videos available to ACBS members).

We hope that you find these free videos useful.  Simply click on the links below to browse.

If you find others on Youtube or elsewhere, please let us know by Adding a Comment and include the URL for the video and we will add it.
 

Community

Applications of RFT to EIBI programs for children with autism

Applications of RFT to EIBI programs for children with autism

Ian Stewart, John McElwee and Siri Ming presented this 3 hour workshop at the Penn State National Autism conference in 2011:

http://wpsu.org/live/archive_player/37438

Jennifer Krafft

Relational Frame Theory (RFT) - Your ACT Auntie Series

Relational Frame Theory (RFT) - Your ACT Auntie Series

Here is a video on RFT from the Your ACT Auntie Series

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=XvnEn1Y-gcQ

Community

Relational Frame Theory: From Basic Science to Behavioral/Clinical application

Relational Frame Theory: From Basic Science to Behavioral/Clinical application

Relational Frame Theory: From Basic Science to Behavioral/Clinical Application


In this presentation, Thomas G. Szabo, Ph.D., BCBA:
• Introduces basic RFT terminology
• Reviews applied RFT findings in the literature
• Suggests essential strategies for using RFT to promote
o Perspective-taking
o Relational responding
o Creativity

 

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=7ViXj2XEY28

admin

Research: Basic & Applied

Research: Basic & Applied

The core of the contextual behavioral science (CBS) model of science is work that is inherently translational in nature; basic research informing interventions and vice versa.

Therefore RFT researchers are comprised of basic scientists who possess an interest in RFT’s application to human behavior, or are also clinicians or applied behavior analysts who are interested in understanding clinically-relevant phenomena. As such, RFT principles and processes have been useful for influencing and optimizing performance and functioning across a broad range of human phenomena.

In the Publications section of this website, you will find articles and books about RFT. 

JCBS had a Special Issue on Conceptual Developments in Relational Frame Theory: Research and Practice; Guest Edited by Dermot Barnes-Holmes, Yvonne Barnes-Holmes, Ian Stewart and Thomas Parling

Also, the ACBS community has has a list of research labs all over the world where you might:

  • gain a better understanding of the current lines of research in RFT that is ongoing;
  • network with others doing similar work as yours;
  • foster collaboration between researchers and labs;
  • follow up with researchers on the status of projects.

Please click here to find a comprehensive list of RFT laboratories.

Community

Empirical Support

Empirical Support

This list includes research articles that contain original data relevant to RFT up until 2010. As the number of RFT articles has increased greatly since then, updating this list has become challenging.

For the most up-to-date list, visit the publications database and choose "RFT: Empirical" as a search term. To search for RFT articles by year type in "RFT 2011" or "Relational 2011" on the publications database search.

When logged in, ACBS members can add child pages to update this with new literature -- learn how here.

As of the beginning of 2009, there are about 150 empirical articles that are either on RFT ideas, or very closely related.

2011 - 2021

Hayes, S. C., Law, S., Assemi, K., Falletta-Cowden, N., Shamblin, M., Burleigh, K., Olla, R., Forman, M., & Smith, P. (2021). Relating is an Operant: A Fly Over of 35 Years of RFT Research. Perspectivas Em Análise Do Comportamento, 12(1). DOI: 10.18761/PAC.2021.v12.RFT.02
RFT was first formally described in 1985 and in the 35 years since, hundreds of studies have been conducted on relational learning from an operant point of view. This paper briefly summarizes that history and examines some of its key claims. So far, the empirical program delineated by RFT has held up remarkably well.

O'Connor, M, Farrell, L, Munnelly, A., and McHugh, L. (2017). Citation analysis of relational frame theory: 2009–2016. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, Volume 6, Issue 2, Pages 152-158.
This article contains a list of Empirical RFT articles in the Supplementary material for Empirical RFT articles

2010

Barnes-Holmes, D., Murphy, A., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I., (2010). The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure: Exploring the impact of private versus public contexts and the response latency criterion on pro-white and anti-black stereotyping among white Irish individuals. The Psychological Record, 60, 57-80.

Barnes-Holmes, D., Murtagh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2010). Using the Implicit Association Test and the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure to measure attitudes towards meat and vegetables in vegetarians and meat-eaters. The Psychological Record.
One of the first two empirical studies to test the validity of the IRAP by comparing it to the IAT using a "known-groups" approach. The studies also introduce the use of the D-IRAP algorithm.

Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & O’Hora, D. (2010). Relational Frame Theory and human intelligence. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 11(1), 37-51.
An early conceptual look at the relationship between derived relational responding and intelligence.

Dymond, S., May, R. J., Munnelly, A., & Hoon, A. E. (2010). Evaluating the evidence based for relational frame theory: A citation analysis. The Behavior Analyst, 33, 97-117. 
A total of 174 articls were identified between 1991 and 2008, 62 (36%) of which were empirical and 112 (64%) were nonempirical articles. Further analyses on these articles are conducted.

Dymond, S., & Roche, B. (2010). The impact of derived relational responding on gambling behavior. Analysis of Gambling Behavior, 4, 1-16.

Gore, N. J., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Murphy, G. (2010). The relationship between intellectual functioning and relational perspective-taking. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 10(1), 1-17.

Guinther, P. M., & Dougher, M. J. (2010). Semantic False Memories In The Form Of Derived Relational Intrusions Following Training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93(3), 329.

Hinton, E. C., Dymond, S, Von Hecker, U. & Evans, C. J. (2010). Neural correlates of relational reasoning and the symbolic distance effect: Involvement of parietal cortex. Neuroscience, 168, 138-148. 

Hooper, N., Saunders, S., & McHugh, L. (2010). The derived generalization of thought suppression. Learning and Behavior.

Levin, M. E., Hayes, S. C., & Waltz, T. (2010). Creating an implicit measure of cognition more suited to applied research: A test of the Mixed Trial - Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (MT-IRAP). International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 6, 245-261. 

Moran, L., Stewart, I., McElwee, J., & Ming, S. (2010). Brief Report: The Training and Assessment of Relational Precursors and Abilities (TARPA): A Preliminary Analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

Munnelly, A., Dymond, S., & Hinton, E. C. (2010). Relational reasoning with derived comparative relations: A novel model of transitive inference. Behavioral Processes, 85, 8-17.

Vahey, N., Boles, S., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2010). Measuring Adolescents' Smoking-related Social Identity Preferences with the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) for the First Time: A Starting Point that Explains Later IRAP Evolutions. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 10, 453-474.

Villatte, M., Monestès, J. L., McHugh, L., Freixa i Baqué, E., & Loas, G. (2010). Adopting the perspective of another in belief attribution: Contribution of Relational Frame Theory to the understanding of impairments in schizophrenia. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41, 125-134.

Villatte, M., Monestès, J. L., McHugh, L., Freixa i Baqué, E., & Loas, G. (2010). Assessing perspective taking in schizophrenia using Relational Frame Theory. The Psychological Record, 60, 413-424.

2009

Barnes-Holmes, D., Waldron, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2009). Testing the validity of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) and the Implicit Association Test (IAT): Measuring attitudes towards Dublin and country life in Ireland. The Psychological Record, 59, 389-406.
One of the first two empirical studies to test the validity of the IRAP by comparing it to the IAT using a "known-groups" approach. The studies also introduce the use of the D-IRAP algorithm.

Dawson, D. L., Barnes-Holmes, D., Gresswell, D. M., Hart, A. J. P., & Gore, N. J. (2009). Assessing the implicit beliefs of sexual offenders using the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure: A First Study. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 21, 57-75.

Power, P. M., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2009). The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a measure of implicit relative preferences: A first study. The Psychological Record, 59, 621-640.
The first IRAP study that involved using comparative relations as a means of determining implicit relative preferences for different social groups. This was also the first study to show a clear divergence between responses on the IRAP and an explicit measure.

Gross, A. C., & Fox, E. J. (2009). Relational frame theory: An overview of the controversy. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 25, 87-98.

Vahey, N. A., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2009). A first test of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a measure of self-esteem: Irish prisoner groups and university students. The Psychological Record, 59, 371-388.
The first empirical study that aimed to develop a self-esteem IRAP.

Lipkens, G. & Hayes, S. C. (2009). Producing and recognizing analogical relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 91, 105-126.
Article shows for the first time that relating relations can be the basis of producing related events, selecting relations, or selecting related events.

Murphy, C. & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2009). Establishing derived manding for specific amounts with three children: An attempt at synthesizing Skinner's verbal behavior and relational frame theory. Psychological Record, 59(1), 75-91.

O'Connor, J., Rafferty, A., Barnes-Holmes, D. & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2009). The role of verbal behavior, stimulus nameability and familiarity on the equivalence performances of autistic and normally developing children.Psychological Record, 59(10), 53-74.

O'Toole, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2009). Three chronometric indices of relational responding as predictors of performance on a brief intelligence test: The importance of relational flexibility. The Psychological Record, 59(1), 119-132.
The first empirical study that used the IRAP to investigate the relationship between relational responding and IQ. Although not reported in the article, the research allowed us to determine that the D-IRAP algorithm appears to control for individual differences in cognitive ability.

2008

Barnes-Holmes, D., Hayden, E., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2008). The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a response-time and event-related-potentials methodology for testing natural verbal relations: A preliminary study. The Psychological Record, 58, 497-516.
The first empirical study of the IRAP. The article also provides a brief potted history of the research that led to the development of the procedure.

Dymond, S., Roche, B., Forsyth, J.P., Whelan, R. & Rhoden, J. (2008). Derived avoidance learning: Transformation of avoidance response functions in accordance with same and opposite relational frames. The Psychological Record, 58, 269-286.
This interesting study demonstrated that participants will demonstrate avoidance based on the stimulus relations of “same” and “opposite.” Control participants provided evidence that the transformation was a direct product of a relational learning history. Although equivalence may account for the finding, the explanation is circuitous.

Gavin, A., Roche, B., & Ruiz, M. R. (2008). Competing contingencies over derived relational responding: A behavioral model of the implicit association test. The Psychological Record, 58, 427-441.

Ju, W. C. & Hayes, S. C. (2008). Verbal establishing stimuli: Testing the motivative effect of stimuli in a derived relation with consequences. The Psychological Record, 58, 339-363.

McHugh, L., & Reed, P. (2008). Using Relational Frame Theory to build grammar in children with Autistic Spectrum Conditions. The Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Applied Behavior Analysis, 2.4-3.1, 60-77.

O'Hora, D., Pelaez, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., Rae, G., Robinson, K., & Chaudhary, T. (2008). Temporal relations and intelligence: Correlating relational performance with performance on the WAIS-III. The Psychological Record, 58, 569-584.

Pérez-González, L.A., Herszlikowicz, K. & Williams, G. (2008). Stimulus relations analysis and the emergence of novel intraverbals. Psychological Record, 58, 95-129.

Roche, B. T., Kanter, J. W., Brown, K. R., Dymond, S., & Fogarty, C. C. (2008). A comparison of "direct" versus "derived" extinction of avoidance responding. The Psychological Record, 58, 443-464.

Roche, B. & Dymond, S. (2008). A transformation of functions in accordance with the nonarbitrary relational properties of sexual stimuli. Psychological Record, 58, 71-94.

Vitale, A., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Campbell, C. (2008). Facilitating responding in accordance with the relational frame of comparison: Systematic empirical analyses. The Psychological Record, 58, 365-390.

Weinstein, J. H., Wilson, K. G., Drake, C. E., & Kellum, K. K. (2008). A Relational Frame Theory Contribution to Social Categorization. Behavior and Social Issues, 17, 39-64.

2007

Berens, N. M., & Hayes, S. C. (2007). Arbitrarily applicable comparative relations: Experimental Evidence for relational operants. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 45-71.
In a combined multiple baseline (across responses and participants) and multiple probe design (with trained and untrained stimuli), it was shown that reinforced multiple exemplar training facilitated the development of arbitrary comparative relations, and that these skills generalized not just across stimuli but also across trial types.

Cahill, J., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Rodríguez-Valverde, M., Luciano, C., & Smeets, P. M. (2007). The derived transfer and reversal of mood functions through equivalence relations II. The Psychological Record, 57, 373-389.

Dougher, M. J., Hamilton, D., Fink, B., & Harrington, J. (2007). Transformation of the discriminative and eliciting functions of generalized relational stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 88(2), 179-197.
Transformation of respondent stimulus functions via more-than/less-than direct and derived relations.

Gómez, S., López, F., Martín, C.B., Barnes-Holmes, Y. & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2007). Exemplar training and a derived transformation of functions in accordance with symmetry and equivalence. Psychological Record, 57(2), 273-294.
This is a very straightforward study programming transformation of function with young children. The first experiment simply exposed four children to action-object exemplar training and tested for symmetry. Experiment two enhanced transformation via multiple exemplar training; effectively demonstrating the ability to program stimulus relations via exemplar training.

Gaynor, S. T., Washio, Y. & Anderson, F. (2007). The conjunction fallacy: A derived stimulus relations conceptualization and demonstration experiment. The Psychological Record, 57, 63-85.

Luciano, M. C., Gómez, I., & Rodríguez, M. (2007). The Role of Multiple-Exemplar Training and Naming in Establishing Derived Equivalence in an Infant. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior.

McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y. & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2007). Deictic relational complexity and the development of deception. Psychological Record, 57(4), 517-531.

McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Whelan, R. & Stewart, I. (2007). Knowing me, knowing you: Deictic complexity in false-belief understanding. Psychological Record, 57(4), 533-542.

McKenna, I. M., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2007). Testing the fake-ability of the implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP): the first study. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, (7)2, 123-138.
The first empirical study that sought to determine if the IRAP can be readily faked.

O'Toole, C., Barnes-Holmes, D. & Smyth, S. (2007). A derived transfer of functions and the Implicit Association Test.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 88(2), 263-283.

Rehfeldt, R., Dillen, J. E., Ziomek, M. M. & Kowalchuk, R. K. (2007). Assessing Relational Learning Deficits in Perspective-Taking in Children with High Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder. The Psychological Record, 57, 23-47.

Rosales, R. & Rehfeldt, R.A. (2007). Contriving transitive conditioned establishing operations to establish derived manding skills in adults with severe developmental disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40(1), 105-121.

Schlund, M.W., Hoehn-Saric, R. & Cataldo, M.F. (2007). New knowledge derived from learned knowledge: Functional-anatomic correlates of stimulus equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87(2), 287-307.

2006

Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Power, P., Hayden, E., Milne, R., & Stewart, I. (2006). Do you really know what you believe? developing the implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP) as a direct measure of implicit beliefs. The Irish Psychologist, (32)7, 169-177.

Dixon, M.R., Rehfeldt, R.A., Zlomke, K.R. & Robinson, A. (2006). Exploring the development and dismantling of equivalence classes involving terrorist stimuli. Psychological Record, 56, 83-103.
This paper describes 2 studies that present a conceptual interpretation and experimental findings involving developing and dismantling of equivalence classes consisting of terrorist stimuli. Results of experiment 1 showed that participants made predictable responses to stimuli during pretest, however made fewer culturally controlled responses after training. Experiment 2 showed that it was easy to acquire relations involving terrorist stimuli when compared to two other conditions. Implications are discussed.

Dixon, M. R., Zlomke, K. M., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2006). Restoring Americans’ Nonequivalent Frames of Terror: An Application of Relational Frame Theory. The Behavior Analyst Today, 7(3), 275-289.

Haas, J. R., & Hayes, S.C. (2006). When knowing you are doing well hinders performance: Exploring the interaction between rules and feedback. Journal of Organizational Behavior Management, 26, 91-111.
The effect of two types of verbal consequences, rule-following feedback and task performance feedback, on rule-induced insensitivity to programmed schedules of reinforcement were examined. Rule-following feedback could be either accurate or non-contingently positive. The task involved moving a sign through a grid using telegraph keys operating on a multiple DRL 6/FR 18 schedule of reinforcement in the presence of an initially accurate rule. After acquisition, the multiple schedule was changed without notice to a FR 1/FI Yoked schedule. Accurate rule-following feedback plus feedback on task performance produced striking insensitivity to the DRL 6 to FR 1 schedule change, the opposite of what might be expected by a common sense analysis of task performance feedback, even after controlling for contact with the changed contingency. It is argued that findings such as these can only be understood by considering the mutual verbal relations evoked by the combinations of rules and feedback, rather than treating feedback as a simple consequential event or as a verbal consequence whose effects do not depend on the relations sustained with other events.

McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D. & Stewart, I. (2006). Understanding false belief as generalized operant behavior. Psychological Record, 56(3), 341-364.

Ninness, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., Rumph, R., McCuller, G., Ford, A. M., Payne, R., et al. (2006). Transformations of mathematical and stimulus functions. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 299-321.

Smeets, P. M., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Striefel, S. (2006). Establishing and reversing equivalence relations with precursor to the relational evaluation procedure. The Psychological Record, 56(2), 267-286.

Smyth, S., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Forsyth, J. P. (2006). A derived transfer of simple discrimination and self-reported arousal functions in spider fearful and non-spider fearful participants. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 85(2), 223-246.
This article used the stimulus paring observation procedure to demonstrate transformation of arousal functions, and provided further evidence that anxiety responses can participate in arbitrary relational frames and produce problematic clinical outcomes.

Valdivia, S., Luciano, C., & Molina, F. J. (2006). Verbal regulation of motivational states. The Psychological Record, 56, 577-595.

Whelan, R., Barnes-Holmes, D. & Dymond, S. (2006). The transformation of consequential functions in accordance with the relational frames of more-than and less-than. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 86(3), 317-335.

2005

Barnes-Holmes, D., Regan, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Commins, S., Walsh, D., Stewart, I., et al. (2005). Relating derived relations as a model of analogical reasoning: reaction times and event-related potentials. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 84(3), 435-451.
This study used event related potentials to test the RFT prediction that same-same relations are simpler and functionally distinct different-different analogical reasoning. Reaction times were significantly longer, and waveforms were significantly more negative for different-different than they were for same-same relations. This is consistent with the prediction based on RFT.

Dixon, M.R. & Zlomke, K.M. (2005). Implementación del precursor del procedimiento de evaluación relacional en el establecimiento de marcos relacionales de igualdad, oposición y diferencia. = Using the precursor to the relational evaluation procedure (PREP) to establish the relational frames of sameness, opposition, and distinction. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 37(2), 305-316.

Gutiérrez-Martí¬nez, O., Luciano-Soriano, M. C., & Valdivia, S. (2005). Change of self-efficacy verbalizations and derivation of functions. Psicothema, 17, 625-630.

Merwin, R.M. & Wilson, Kelly G. (2005). Preliminary findings on the effects of self-referring and evaluative stimuli on stimulus equivalence class formation. Psychological Record, 55, 561-575.

Murphy, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2005). Derived manding in children with autism: Synthesizing Skinner's verbal behavior with relational frame theory. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38(4), 445-462.

Ninness, C., Rumph, R., McCuller, G., Harrison, C., Ford, A. M., & Ninness, S. K. (2005). A functional analytic approach to computer-interactive mathematics. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38 , 1-22.
One of the first uses of RFT in higher education. Here is the abstract: Following a pretest, 11 participants who were naive with regard to various algebraic and trigonometric transformations received an introductory lecture regarding the fundamentals of the rectangular coordinate system. Following the lecture, they took part in a computer-interactive matching-to-sample procedure in which they received training on particular formula-to-formula and formula-to-graph relations as these formulas pertain to reflections and vertical and horizontal shifts. In training A-B, standard formulas served as samples and factored formulas served as comparisons. In training B-C, factored formulas served as samples and graphs served as comparisons. Subsequently, the program assessed for mutually entailed B-A and C-B relations as well as combinatorially entailed C-A and A-C relations. After all participants demonstrated mutual entailment and combinatorial entailment, we employed a test of novel relations to assess 40 different and complex variations of the original training formulas and their respective graphs. Six of 10 participants who completed training demonstrated perfect or near-perfect performance in identifying novel formula-to-graph relations. Three of the 4 participants who made more than three incorrect responses during the assessment of novel relations showed some commonality among their error patterns. Derived transfer of stimulus control using mathematical relations is discussed.

Ninness, C., Rumph, R., McCuller, G., Vasquez III, E., Harrison, C., Ford, A.M., et al. (2005b). A relational frame and artificial neural network approach to computer-interactive mathematics. Psychological Record, 55, 135-153.

O'Hora, D., Â Pelaez, M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2005). Derived relational responding and performance on verbal subtests of the WAIS-III. The Psychological Record, 55(1), 155-175.

Rehfeldt, R.A. & Dymond, S. (2005). The effects of test order and nodal distance on the emergence and stability of derived discriminative stimulus functions. Psychological Record, 55(2), 179-196.

Rehfeldt, R. A. & Root, S. L. (2005). Establishing derived requesting skills in adults with severe developmental disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 101-105.

Reilly, T., Whelan, R., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2005). The effect of training structure on the latency responses to a five-term linear chain. The Psychological Record, 55(2), 233-249.

Smeets, P.M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2005a). Auditory-visual and visual-visual equivalence relations in children. The Psychological Record, 55(3), 483-503.

Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes D. (2005b). Establishing equivalence classes in preschool children with many-to-one and one-to-many training protocols. Behavioural Processes, 69(3), 281-293.

Whelan, R., Cullinan, V. & O'Donovan, A. (2005). Derived same and opposite relations produce association and mediated priming. = Mismas Relaciones derivadas y Opuestas Producen la Asociación y la Preparación Mediada.International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 5(3), 247-264.

2004

Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (2004). Establishing relational responding in accordance with opposite as generalized operant behavior in young children. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4, 559-586.

Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Smeets, P. M., & Luciano, C. (2004). The derived transfer of mood functions through equivalence relations. The Psychological Record, 54, 95-114.

Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Smeets, P. M., Strand, P., & Friman, P. (2004). Establishing relational responding in accordance with more-than and less-than as generalized operant behavior in young children.International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4, 531-558.

Barnes-Holmes, D., Staunton, C., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Whelan, R., Stewart, I., Commins, S., et al. (2004). Interfacing Relational Frame Theory with cognitive neuroscience: Semantic priming, The Implicit Association Test, and event related potentials. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4, 215-240.

Carpentier, F., Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004). Equivalence-equivalence: Matching stimuli with same discriminative functions. The Psychological Record, 54, 145-162.

Carpentier, F., Smeets, P. M., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Stewart, I. (2004). Matching derived functionally-same relations: Equivalence-equivalence and classical analogies. The Psychological Record, 54, 255-273.

McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004). Perspective-taking as relational responding: A developmental profile. The Psychological Record, 54, 115-144.

This article describes traditional tasks that relate to what developmental literature calls “Theory of Mind. ” Its goals were to develop and test a protocol across groups in several different developmental stages (young children through adulthood) that indicated that perspective taking could be described in terms of relational responding (arbitrarily applicable, mutually entailed, combinatorily entailed, and showing transformation of stimulus function) and that added but did not contradict the traditional and developmental literature. Study 1 tested the protocol, Study 2 tested whether young children’s poor response was an artifact of word length, and Study 3 tested whether the experimenter’s cues affected responding. Results from three studies indicated indeed that perspective taking can be viewed as an operant, and that deictic frames across three levels of complexity were functionally distinct classes of behavior. A developmental profile emerged, showing that derived relational responding develops with age as well as relational complexity. Further, I-YOU relations emerge before HERE-THERE and NOW-THEN relations, and NOW-THEN relations produced the most errors in all participants regardless of age.

O'Hora, D., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. M. (2004). Derived relational networks and control by novel instructions: A possible model of generative verbal responding. The Psychological Record, 54, 437-460.
Results from two studies examining instructional control on novel stimulus situations are described. The researchers employed the Relational Evaluation Procedure to train relations and then tested these in novel situations. Results support the idea that novel instructions can control behavior and that RFT provides an adequate model for the generativity of language.

Roche, B., Linehan, C., Ward, T., Dymond, S., & Rehfeldt, R. (2004). The Unfolding of the Relational Operant: A Real-time Analysis Using Electroencephalography and Reaction Time Measures. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 4(3), 587-603.

Valverde, M., Luciano, M. C., Gutiérrez Martí nez, O., & Hernández López, M. (2004). Tranfer of latent inhibition of aversively conditioned autonomic responses through equivalence classes. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4, 605-622.

Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2004). A functional-analytic model of analogy using the relational evaluation procedure. The Psychological Record, 54, 531-552.
This study provides an empirical demonstration of analogy using the Relational Evaluation Procedure (REP), a recently developed technique for the rapid training and testing of derived stimulus relations. The experiment involved 9 stages in which 5 adult male subjects were exposed to a complex series of REP training and testing protocols, by the end of which they each readily demonstrated 24 completely novel instances of responding in accordance with analogical relations as conceptualized by RFT.

Smeets, P. M., van Wijngaarden, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Cullinan, V. (2004). Assessing stimulus equivalence with a precursor to the relational evaluation procedure. Behavioural Processes, 65, 241-251.

Whelan, R., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004a). The transformation of consequential functions in accordance with the relational frames of same and opposite. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 82, 177-195.
Formative augmenting, behavior due to relational networks that establish given consequences as reinforcers or as punishers, was demonstrated in accordance with Same and Opposite relational networks. Some stimuli acquired reinforcing functions, based on the derived relation of Opposite, although in some cases no such function had actually been established for any member of the network. These effects were also observed across ABA reversals in the baseline contingencies.

Whelan, R., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004b). Empirical models of formative augmenting in accordance with the relations of same, opposite, more-than, and less-than. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4, 285-302.
Authors demonstrated through two experiments that stimuli acquired reinforcing functions based on derived relational networks. This research supports the idea of formative augmenting: the degree to which events function as consequences was altered based on relational networks of Same and Opposite and more-than and less-than.

2003

Carpentier, F., Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2003a). Equivalence-equivalence as a model of analogy: Further analyses. The Psychological Record, 53, 349-372.
This study attempted to train equivalence-equivalence relations with 5-year-old children. Only 8 of 18 showed equivalence-equivalence relations when tested. The procedure was then tested with adults and was successful with all of them.

Carpentier, F., Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2003b). Matching unrelated stimuli with same discriminative functions: Training order effects. Behavioural Processes, 60, 215-226.

Carr, D. (2003). Effects of exemplar training in exclusion responding on auditory-visual discrimination tasks with children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 507-524.
Showed that multiple-exemplar training with auditory-visual exclusion tasks facilitated nonreinforced exclusion performances which reduced error rates on subsequent novel stimulus sets.

Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2003). Children's emergent preferences for soft drinks: Stimulus-equivalence and transfer. Journal of Economic Psychology, 24, 603-618.

Smeets, P. M., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Akpinar, D. & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2003). Reversal of equivalence relations. The Psychological Record, 53, 91-120.

2002

Carpentier, F., Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2002a). Establishing transfer of compound control in children: A stimulus control analysis. The Psychological Record, 52, 139-158.

Carpentier, F., Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2002b). Matching functionally-same relations: Implications for equivalence-equivalence as a model for analogical reasoning. The Psychological Record, 52, 351-312.
This study tested equivalence-equivalence and nonequivalence-nonequivalence relations with adults, 9-year-olds, and 5-year-olds. Most of the adults and 9-year-olds demonstrated these relations, but the 5-year-olds did not.

Dougher, M., Perkins, D. R., Greenway, D., Koons, A., & Chiasson, C. (2002). Contextual control of equivalence-based transformation of functions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 63-94.
Transformation of functions among members in equivalence classes

Gomez, S., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Luciano, M. C. (2002). Generalized break equivalence II: Contextual control over a generalized pattern of stimulus relations. The Psychological Record, 52, 203-220.
Building on the work of Gomez et al (1999; 2001), the authors established effective contextual control over the Generalised Break Equivalence Pattern (GBEP) which provides further support for the generalized operant nature of derived relational responding.

Luciano, C., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2002). Establishing reports of saying and doing and discriminations of say-do relations. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 23, 406-421.

Markham, M.R., Dougher, M.J. & Augustson, E.M. (2002). Transfer of operant discrimination and respondent elicitation via emergent relations of compound stimuli. Psychological Record, 52(3), 325-350.

O'Hora, D., Roche, B., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P. (2002). Response latencies to multiple derived stimulus relations: Testing two predictions of Relational Frame Theory. The Psychological Record, 52, 51-75.
The authors measured response latencies to mutually entailed same, opposite, more-than, and less-than relations. Response latencies to same and opposite relations were significantly faster than more-than and less-than relations. A second experiment showed a gradual decrease in response latency for more/less relations across a novel stimulus set.

Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B., & Smeets, P. M. (2002a). Stimulus equivalence and non-arbitrary relations. The Psychological Record, 52, 77-88

Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. M. (2002b). A functional-analytic model of analogy: A relational frame analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 375-396.
This study explored a behavior-analytic model of analogical reasoning, defined as the discrimination of formal similarity via equivalence-equivalence responding. Equivalence classes were trained, and subjects responded according to equivalence-equivalence relations. Subjects discriminated by shape or color of a relata. Transformation of stimulus functions of a block-sorting task based on this model of analogy was also shown.

2001

Barnes-Holmes, Y. Barnes-Holmes, D. Roche, B, & Smeets, P. M. (2001a). Exemplar training and a derived transformation of function in accordance with symmetry: I. The Psychological Record, 51, 287- 308.
This paper demonstrates that multiple exemplar training provides the necessary history to establish transformation across symmetry relations. Four studies confirmed that after relatively few exemplar training sessions, transformation could occur even across response modalities.

Barnes-Holmes, Y. Barnes-Holmes, D. Roche, B, & Smeets, P. M. (2001b). Exemplar training and a derived transformation of function in accordance with symmetry II. The Psychological Record, 51, 589-603.
This study clears some questions raised by Part I. Specifically, the authors found that naming is not a critical component of transformation, and children could readily demonstrate transformed functions in accordance with symmetry with no history of naming. The third part of the study examined the effects of pre-training. Results suggest that pre-training may be effective, but if not, to forgo additional pre-training and move immediately to multiple exemplar training.

Cullinan, V. A., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (2001). A precursor to the relational evaluation procedure: Searching for the contextual cues that control equivalence responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 76, 339-349.

Fitzgerald, D. L. (2001). The effects of fluency in the acquisition of conditional, symmetric, and equivalence relations on the emergence of derived relational responding and the contextual control of relational behavior. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 61(9-B).

Gomez, S., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Luciano, M. C. (2001). Generalized break equivalence I. The Psychological Record, 51, 131-150.
The objective of this study was to produce responding in accordance with symmetry and transitivity but not with equivalence across novel stimulus sets. Building on the work of Gomez et al (1999), the authors employed several new procedures to generate ‘broken’ equivalence relations which provides support for the generalized operant nature of derived relational responding.

Leader, G., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2001). Establishing fraction-decimal equivalence using a respondent-type training procedure. The Psychological Record, 51, 151-166.

Leader, G., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2001). Matching-to-sample and respondent-type training as methods for producing equivalence relations: Isolating the critical variable. The Psychological Record, 51, 429-444.

Luciano, M. C., Herruzo, J., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2001). Generalization of say-do correspondence. The Psychological Record, 51, 111-130.

Lyddy, F., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Hampson, P. J. (2001). A transfer of sequence function via equivalence in a connectionist network. The Psychological Record, 51, 409-428.

O'Hora, D., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Developing a procedure to model the establishment of instructional control. Experimental Analysis of Behavior Bulletin, 19, 13-15.
The authors measured response latencies to mutually entailed same, opposite, more-than, and less-than relations. Response latencies to same and opposite relations were significantly faster than more-than and less-than relations. A second experiment showed a gradual decrease in response latency for more/less relations across a novel stimulus set.]

Smeets, P. M., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Derived stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus relations in children and adults: Assessing training order effects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 78, 130-154.

Osborne, J.G. & Koppel, L. (2001). Acquisition, generalization, and contextual control of taxonomic and thematic relational responding. Psychological Record, 51(2), 185-205.

Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. M. (2001). Generating derived relational networks via the abstraction of common physical properties: A possible model of analogical reasoning. The Psychological Record, 51, 381-408.
This study demonstrated equivalence-equivalence responding based on the abstraction of common formal properties.

2000

Augustson, E.M., Dougher, M.J. & Markham, M.R. (2000). Emergence of conditional stimulus relations and transfer of respondent eliciting functions among compound stimuli. Psychological Record, 50(4), 745-770.
Transfer of respondent eliciting functions occurs even among compound stimuli.

Barnes-Holmes, D., Keane, J., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Smeets, P. M. (2000). A derived transformation of emotive functions as a means of establishing differential preferences for soft drinks. The Psychological Record, 50, 493-511.

Carpentier, F., Smeets, P. M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2000). Matching compound samples with unitary compounds: Derived stimulus relations in adults and children. The Psychological Record, 50, 671-686.

Carr, D., Wilkinson, K. M., Blackman, D., & McIlvane, W. J. (2000). Equivalence classes in individuals with minimal verbal repertoires. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 101-115.

Cullinan, V. A., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (2000). A precursor to the relational evaluation procedure. II.The Psychological Record, 50, 467-492.

Healy, O., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P.M. (2000). Derived relational responding as generalised operant behaviour. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 74, 207-227.
Demonstrated that response patterns on novel stimulus sets was controlled by the feedback delivered for previous stimulus sets.

Leader, G., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (2000). Establishing equivalence relations using a respondent-type training procedure III. The Psychological Record, 50, 63-78.
Similar to the study above but with young children as subjects.

Roche, B., Barnes-Holmes, D., Smeets, P. M., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & McGeady, S. (2000). Contextual control over the derived transformation of discriminative and sexual arousal functions. The Psychological Record, 50, 267-291.
Following on from Roche and Barnes (1997) and McGeady and Roche (1997), the authors demonstrate four distinct contextually-controlled transformations of function by presenting the contextual cue along with the derived stimuli. Skin resistance responses and operant discriminations are measured. Still the only study to employ such a testing format.

Smeets, P.M., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Cullinan, V. (2000). Establishing equivalence classes with match-to-sample format and simultaneous-discrimination format conditional discrimination tasks. The Psychological Record, 50, 721-744.

Smeets, P. M., Dymond, S., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2000). Instructions, stimulus equivalence, and stimulus sorting: Effects of sequential testing arrangements and a default option. The Psychological Record, 50, 339-354.

1999

Gomez, S., Huerta, F., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Luciano, C. (1999). Breaking equivalence relations. Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin, 17, 1-4.
The objective of this study was to produce responding in accordance with symmetry and transitivity but not with equivalence, across novel stimulus sets.

Smeets, P. M., Barnes, D., & Luciano, C. M. (1999). Reversal of emergent simple discrimination in children: A component analysis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60(3), 327-343.

1998

Cullinan, V. A., Barnes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (1998). A precursor to the relational evaluation procedure: Analyzing stimulus equivalence.The Psychological Record, 48, 121-145.

Dymond S. & Barnes D. (1998). The effects of prior equivalence testing and verbal instructions on derived self-discrimination transfer: A follow-up study. Psychological Record 48(1), 147-170.
A follow-up study to Dymond & Barnes (1994). Subjects were not given equivalence training this time but were instead trained in a series of conditional discriminations. Also, some subjects were given extensive instructions and others were given minimal instructions. Neither of these factors affected the subjects performance and the results from previous experiments were replicated.

Hayes, S. C., & Bissett, R. (1998). Derived stimulus relations produce mediated and episodic priming. The Psychological Record, 48, 617-630.
Showed that priming effects that are well known in semantically related words also occurred in nonsense stimuli related through equivalence.

Healy, O., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P.M. (1998). Derived relational responding as an operant: The effects of between-session feedback. The Psychological Record, 48, 511-536.
Delivering accurate or inaccurate feedback to subjects following a test for derived equivalence relations produces responding on subsequent tests that is consistent with that feedback. One of the first to demonstrate the operant nature of relation responding.

1997

Barnes, D., Hegarty, N., & Smeets, P. M. (1997). Relating equivalence relations to equivalence relations: A relational framing model of complex human functioning. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 14, 57-83.
This study examined the RFT approach to analogical reasoning. Subjects were trained on several equivalence relations. They were then shown pairs of relata in which both of the relata were from the same relation or in which both relata were from different relations. The subjects successfully matched pairs of same with same and different with different.

McGeady, S. & Roche, B. (1997). A contextually controlled transformation of operant response functions in accordance with arbitrarily applicable relations. Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin, 15, 12-13.

Roche, B., & Barnes, D. (1997). A transformation of respondently conditioned sexual arousal functions in accordance with arbitrary relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 67, 275-301.
The first study to show a transformation of respondently conditioned sexual arousal functions, measured as skin resistance responses, through same and opposite relations. An excellent demonstration of how to conduct complex electrodermal research within an RFT framework.

Roche, B., Barnes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (1997). Incongruous stimulus pairing and conditional discrimination training: Effects on relational responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 68, 143-160.

Smeets, P., & Barnes, D. (1997). Emergent conditional discrimination in children and adults: Stimulus equivalence derived from simple discriminations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 64-84.

Smeets, P., Barnes, D., & Roche, B. (1997). Functional equivalence in children. Derived stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus relations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 1-17.

Smeets, P.M., Leader, G., & Barnes, D. (1997). Establishing stimulus classes with adults and children using a respondent training procedure: A follow-up study. The Psychological Record, 47, 285-308.

1996

Barnes, D., Lawlor, H., Smeets, P. M., & Roche, B. (1996). Stimulus equivalence and academic self-concept in mildly mentally handicapped and non-mentally handicapped children. The Psychological Record, 46, 87-107.
Using educationally-relevant real world stimuli such as "slow" and "able" as well as the subject's own name, the authors show how developmentally-delayed children come to fail tests for equivalence when the predicted outcome is in contrast to their learning history. That is, subjects did not relate their own name to "able." A neat study on prior-learning effects in equivalence formation.

Dymond, S. & Barnes, D. (1996). A transformation of self-discrimination response functions in accordance with the arbitrarily applicable relations of sameness and opposition. The Psychological Record, 46, 271-300.
Demonstrates a transformation of functions in accordance with sameness and opposition, using several matching-to-sample control tasks to prevent formation of simple equivalence and nonequivalence relations.

Grey, I., & Barnes, D. (1996). Stimulus equivalence and attitudes. The Psychological Record, 46, 243-270.

Leader, G., Barnes, D., & Smeets, P.M. (1996). Establishing equivalence relations using a respondent-type training procedure. The Psychological Record, 46, 685-706.
The first in a series of studies investigating a new procedure for the derivation of equivalence relations. “Training” merely involves observing on-screen presentations of stimulus pairs and then testing for equivalence using a match-to-sample format. More effective in establishing equivalence than standard MTS arrangements.

Roche, B., & Barnes, D. (1996). Arbitrarily applicable relational responding and sexual categorization: A critical test of the difference relation. The Psychological Record, 46, 451-475.
After Steele & Hayes, the first study to systematically examine the relational frame of distinction using socially-loaded stimuli. This study inspired a series of exchanges between the authors and Richard Saunders in the same volume on the relationship between equivalence and RFT.

Smeets, P., Barnes, D., & Schenk, J., & Darcheville, J., (1996). Emergent simple discriminations and conditional relations in children, adults with mental retardation, and normal adults. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49(B), 201-219.

Wilson, K. G. & Hayes, S. C. (1996). Resurgence of derived stimulus relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 66, 267-281.

1995

Barnes, D., Browne, M., Smeets, P., & Roche, B. (1995). A transfer of functions and a conditional transfer of functions through equivalence relations in three- to six-year-old children. The Psychological Record, 45, 405-430.
Transfer and contextually-controlled transfer in kids of different ages with the older subjects passing the more complex tests. A nice example of a nonautomated transfer study.

Dymond, S. & Barnes, D. (1995). A transformation of self-discrimination response functions in accordance with the arbitrarily applicable relations of sameness, more-than, and less-than. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, 163-184. Erratum, 66, 348.
The first study to show three patterns of derived relational responding in accordance with sameness, more-than, and less-than. Alternative explanations for the transformation test outcomes are considered and found wanting. The relational network figure has been reproduced in several different publications

Smeets, P. M., & Barnes, D. (1995). Emergent simple discrimination via transfer from differentially reinforced S+ stimuli: A further test of the stimulus-response interaction model. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48B(4), 329-345.

Smeets, P., Barnes, D., & Luciano, C. (1995). Total reversal of emergent simple discrimination in children: A component analysis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 327-343.

Smeets, P., Schenk, J., & Barnes, D. (1995). Establishing arbitrary stimulus classes via identity matching training and non-reinforced matching with complex stimuli. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48B(4), 311-328.

1994

Barnes, D. (1994). Stimulus equivalence and relational frame theory. The Psychological Record, 44, 91-124.
A cogent introduction to RFT in which the author compares "Sidman equivalence" with RFT, offers a respondent analysis of symmetry, and predicts various outcomes of training designs. Good for an undergraduate introduction to the area

Cullinan, V., Barnes, D., Hampson, P. J., & Lyddy, F. (1994). A transfer of explicitly and nonexplicitly trained sequence responses through equivalence relations: An experimental demonstration and connectionist model. The Psychological Record, 44, 559-585.

Dougher, M. J., Augustson, E., Markham, M. R., Greenway, D. E., & Wulfert, E. (1994). The transfer of respondent eliciting and extinction functions through stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 62, 331-351.
One of the early articles studying the transfer of respondent eliciting functions.

Dymond, S., & Barnes, D. (1994). A transfer of self-discrimination response functions through equivalence relations.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 62, 251-267.
Four subjects were trained in matching-to-sample tasks and equivalence relations. They showed the expected transfer of self-discrimination response functions. Four control subjects either received training in matching to sample but were not tested on equivalence or were trained and tested using stimuli not used in the transfer test. None of these showed the transfer of self-discrimination response functions.

Hayes, L. J., Brenner, K., & Hayes, S. C. (1994). Assessing pre-existing stimulus relations via stimulus equivalence.Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 20, 146-166.

1993

Barnes, D., & Hampson, P. (1993). Stimulus equivalence and connectionism: Implications for behavior analysis and cognitive science. The Psychological Record, 43, 617-638.

Barnes, D., & Keenan, M. (1993). A transfer of functions through derived arbitrary and non-arbitrary stimulus relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 61-81.
An elegant demonstration of transfer of functions through equivalence relations, with and without a prior equivalence test, and a generalised transfer through non-arbitrary relations. One of the most-cited transfer articles.

Lipkens, R., Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1993). Longitudinal study of the development of derived relations in an infant. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 201-239.
Showed the development of derived stimulus relations, including equivalence and exclusion, in a human infant.

Schusterman, R. J. & Kastak, D. (1993). A California sea lion (Zalophus californianus) is capable of forming equivalence relations. The Psychological Record, 43, 823-840.

Zentall, T. R., & Urcuioli, P. J. (1993). Emergent relations in the formation of stimulus classes in pigeons. The Psychological Record, 43, 795-810.

1991

Hayes, S. C., Kohlenberg, B. K., & Hayes, L. J. (1991). The transfer of specific and general consequential functions through simple and conditional equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56,119-137.
Showed the transfer of consequential functions through equivalence relations, both simple and conditional.

Kohlenberg, B. S., Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1991). The transfer of contextual control over equivalence classes through equivalence classes: A possible model of social stereotyping. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 505-518.
Showed that transfer effects extended to conditional stimuli that themselves regulated derived relational responding. Extends the analysis to social stereotyping

Steele, D., & Hayes, S. C. (1991). Stimulus equivalence and arbitrarily applicable relational responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 519-555.
The first experimental demonstration that establishing cues that controlled non-arbitrary stimulus relations later produced multiple forms of derived relational responding with arbitrary stimulus sets. One of the first clear experimental demonstrations of RFT.

Watt, A., Keenan, M., Barnes, D., & Cairns, E. (1991). Social categorization and stimulus equivalence. The Psychological Record, 41, 33-50.
This study examined whether social categorization could be explored in terms of stimulus equivalence by testing whether equivalence training could be transferred to untrained social stimuli. The study had Irish Protestants, Irish Catholics and English Protestants go through a series of matching-to-sample procedures in which they were trained to match Protestant or Catholic stimuli with non-sense syllables. The findings suggest that previous learning might interfere with equivalence responding in the experimental training.

1990 and earlier

Barnes, D., McCullagh, P. D., & Keenan, M. (1990). Equivalence class formation in non-hearing impaired children and hearing impaired children. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8, 19-30.

Hayes, L. J., Thompson, S., & Hayes, S. C. (1989). Stimulus equivalence and rule following. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 275-291.
Authors describe results of two studies attempting to specify the relationship between verbal and nonverbal behavior in the context of rule following. Two stimulus equivalence relationships were trained and then subjects were tested for production of novel behavior. Novel behavior was produced in both the presence and absence of names provided for the equivalence classes.

De Rose, J. C., McIlvane, W. J., Dube, W. V., Galpin, V. C., & Stoddard, L. T. (1988). Emergent simple discrimination established by indirect relation to differential consequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 1-20.
One of the early articles studying the transfer of stimulus functions among members in equivalent classes.

Wulfert, E., & Hayes, S. C. (1988). Transfer of a conditional ordering response through conditional equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 125-144.

Hayes, S. C., Brownstein, A. J., Devany, J. M., Kohlenberg, B. S., & Shelby, J. (1987). Stimulus equivalence and the symbolic control of behavior. Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 13, 361-374.
Showed the transfer of discriminative functions through equivalence relations

Devany, J. M., Hayes, S. C., & Nelson, R. O. (1986). Equivalence class formation in language-able and language-disabled children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 243-257.
Showed a correlation between receptive language skills and the derivation of equivalence. Interpreted this correlation in RFT terms, suggesting that the correlation was due to the functional overlap of the two tasks.
 

Steven Hayes

Derived Relations Task With a Chimpanzee (video)

Derived Relations Task With a Chimpanzee (video)

http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=NcgI-rxJOP8

Description: This is an old clip that's been used in a number of introductory RFT / derived relations slideshows. The video shows a chimpanzee having great difficulty in her efforts to solve a bidirectional task (after having learned a correspondence between different people and different letters on a keyboard, she has to «say» who goes to who by using a symbol meaning «to go to»).

admin

RFT Research Archives

RFT Research Archives

Community

Applied RFT

Applied RFT

The most obvious form of "applied" RFT is Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. However, ACT is only one small portion of the applied work being done that utilizes RFT concepts.

Indeed, the principles of RFT and procedures that RFT researchers are honing can be widely applicable to learning, education, child and adult development, clinical problems, and much more.

Below are some areas in which RFT is being used to predict and influence complex human phenomena and to help improve interventions in many clinically relevant areas.

RFT and Perspective Taking

Perspective-taking is a word that refers to a common experience in every one’s life. Behavioral scientists have talked about perspective-taking in many different ways, but the field tends to conceptualize that skill as an intrinsic capacity very often not susceptible to direct manipulation or training, which from a functional contextual point of view is not very useful. Additionally, RFT links this skill to other diverse psychological phenomena such as intelligence or experiential avoidance and it does it from a unique and theoretically coherent framework.

Nowadays, although the RFT started with work on children and kids with developmental disabilities, the literature is expanding to phenomena such as stigma (see Vilardaga et al. 2008 ppt), social anhedonia and psychosis (see Villatte et al, papers in the publication section). Perspective taking is a central aspect of what makes us most human: our ability to interact effectively with other human beings and to form groups, communities, countries and coalitions of countries. Work on deictic framing is a good example of the broad applicability of behavioral principles to human affairs.

RFT and Intervention in Autism Spectrum Disorder

A key aspect of language, and one that is at the core of communication deficits for children with autism, is generativity—put simply, the ability to produce or understand totally new sentences. Understanding and accounting for linguistic generativity is critical to any account of language development (Malott, 2003), and to the creation of programs for teaching flexible and fully functional language repertoires. RFT provides new insight into the issue of generativity, by conceptualizing the core skill in language as learned contextually controlled relational responding (referred to as relational framing).

Typically developing children learn relational framing through natural language interactions during which they are exposed to contingencies that establish these response patterns (e.g., Lipkens, Hayes & Hayes, 1993; Luciano, Gómez & Rodríguez, 2007). However, children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) do not easily learn this key form of responding (e.g., Rehfeldt, Dillen, Ziomek, & Kowalchuk, 2007) —while many children with ASD are able to learn functional language skills through explicit training, language use for many remains rote, despite intensive intervention (see, e.g., Luciano, Rodriguez, Manas, Ruiz, Berens, & Valdivia-Salas, 2009).

A number of recently published RFT-based studies, however, (e.g., Murphy, Barnes-Holmes & Barnes-Holmes, 2005; O’Connor, Rafferty, Barnes-Holmes & Barnes-Holmes, 2009) have begun to show that relational framing can be successfully trained in developmentally delayed populations including individuals with ASD. This work holds great promise for the future.

 REFERENCES

  • Lipkens, R., Hayes, S.C., & Hayes, L.J. (1993). Longitudinal study of the development of derived relations in an infant. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 201-239.
  • Luciano, C., Gomez Becerra, I. & Rodriguez Valverde, M. (2007). The role of multiple- exemplar training and naming in establishing derived equivalence in an infant. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87, 349-365.
  • Luciano, C., Rodriguez, M., Manas, I., Ruiz, F., Berens, N., & Valdivia-Salas, S. (2009). Acquiring the earliest relational operants: coordination, distinction, opposition, comparison and hierarchy. In Rehfeldt, R.A. & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (Eds.). Derived Relational Responding: Applications for Learners with Autism and other Developmental Disabilities. CA: New Harbinger.
  • Malott, R. W. (2003). Behavior analysis and linguistic productivity. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 19, 11-18.
  • Murphy, C., Barnes-Holmes, D. & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2005). Derived manding in children with autism: Synthesizing Skinner's Verbal Behavior with relational frame theory. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 59(1), 445-462.
  • O’Connor, J., Rafferty, A., Barnes-Holmes, D. & Barnes-Holmes , Y. (2009). The role of verbal behavior, stimulus nameability and familiarity on the equivalence performances of autistic and normally-developing children. The Psychological Record, 59(1), 53-74.
  • Rehfeldt, R.A. & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2009). Derived Relational Responding: Applications for Learners with Autism and other Developmental Disabilities. CA: New Harbinger.
  • Rehfeldt, R. A., Dillen, J. E., Ziomek, M. M., & Kowalchuk, R. E. (2007). Assessing relational learning deficits in perspective-taking in children with high functioning autism spectrum disorder. The Psychological Record, 57, 23-47.

 Additional information will be added soon.

Jen Plumb

Research Procedures & Computer Programming

Research Procedures & Computer Programming

This section of the site is for researchers to share custom computer programs, files, instructions, information, and data useful for running computer-controlled experimental procedures. It is our hope that in sharing such information we can develop best practices to reduce the influence of extraneous variables on the results of computer-controlled RFT studies. Please share with the community what you and your lab do.

Disclaimer: All software and files provided on this site are distributed on an "as is" basis. The webmaster or authors of the programs assume no responsibility for problems or damage resulting from the use of these products.
Jen Plumb

Features of Well-Controlled Experimental Analogs

Features of Well-Controlled Experimental Analogs

There have been a number of tightly controlled experimental studies, both on RFT and ACT processes. There are resources within the RFT section of the site that will help you with your own studies, but there are a few that are currently posted to the ACT section of the site that you might find useful as well.

Please visit the Component Studies Information page for information on features of experimental analogs and sample files (visual basic command code, videos, instructions, etc.) from an ACT component study.

Jen Plumb

GO-IRAP Software and Manual

GO-IRAP Software and Manual

Welcome to the Ghent-Odysseus Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (GO-IRAP)! 

 

The GO-IRAP is a new version of the IRAP written in JAVA so it is easily installed, and the new user-friendly interface makes an IRAP very easy to set up.
 
Some new features of the GO-IRAP include:
  • Training and Traditional Testing IRAPs
  • Traditional (Label and Target) and Natural Language (Sentence) IRAP formats 
  • The easy input of complex languages and characters for stimuli and instructions 
  • The use of text or images as labels, targets, and response options
  • Block level or trial-type level application of mastery criteria
  • Label-Target-Response Option stimuli locking 
  • Incorrect and Correct responding feedback options
  • One easily readable .txt data and experimental file 

The 2 program files (32 bit and 64 bit versions) and the PDF of the manual are attached for all ACBS members to download. 

We are also attaching a folder named 'Training IRAP' which was recently used at a workshop presented at the Division of Behavior Analysis Conference in Dublin to demonstrate how the Training IRAP can easily be used in ABA settings to teach both basic and complex relational skills (Barnes-Holmes, McEnteggart, & Kavanagh, 2017). This folder also contains a paper entitled 'Teaching Important Relational Skills for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Intellectual Disability Using Freely Available (GO-IRAP) Software'  which followed on from this workshop (Murphy & Barnes-Holmes, 2017).

Note: Bug Testing. We have been using the GO-IRAP in our research lab without encountering any problems, however, bug testing of the GO-IRAP is ongoing, so no guarantees can be made that no bugs or errors can be found within the program.

Note: Source Code. Because the GO-IRAP is still in the testing phase and there are additional features that we plan to add to the final version, we cannot make the source code available at this time, however we intend to make the source code open access in late 2020.

Also see the https://go-rft.com/ website!

ciara.mcenteggart

Mixed Trial IRAP Software

Mixed Trial IRAP Software

An introduction to the Mixed Trial Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (MT-IRAP)

We designed this program to hopefully provide a more flexible and sensitive tool for examining implicit relational responding. In particular, we are interested in developing/refining an implicit measure that is sensitive and reliable for measuring implicit relational responding at the level of individual stimuli and individual participants (as opposed to implicit effects at the level of stimulus categories and participant groups).

Preliminary data suggests the MT-IRAP can be used to measure implicit relational responding at both the stimulus category and individual stimulus level (Levin, Hayes & Waltz, 2010). It is unclear whether this measure is sensitive and reliable at the level of individual participants and whether it is more or less effective than the standard IRAP. Further research is needed to determine the reliability and validity of this measure, particularly with more ambiguous stimuli (e.g., where participants may be unsure of what is “truth” and what is a “lie”).

At this point, the exact configuration of the MT-IRAP is somewhat of a moving target and depends on the specific research questions being asked. Due to this, the program we have created provides a wide variety of configuration options. The flexibility in this program will hopefully be helpful to you as a researcher, though it may also require more time figuring out how to use it and the most appropriate configuration for a given study.

Please log in and download the MT-IRAP instructions (attached to the bottom of this page), this document contains a great deal more information about the procedures strengths and limitations.

Also, for further information about the MT-IRAP, we’d recommend you read the following article:
Levin, M.E., Hayes, S.C. & Waltz, T. (2010). Creating an implicit measure of cognition more suited to applied research: A test of the Mixed Trial – Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (MT-IRAP). International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 6, 245-262.

Disclaimer: This program is still being developed so you may run into errors while using it. Although we have tried to make it user friendly when possible, due to limited resources and the complexity of the program, it does require some time to get used to using.

Michael Levin

National University of Ireland, Maynooth IRAP software

National University of Ireland, Maynooth IRAP software

The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedures (IRAP)

This software has been used extensively to assess implicit relational responding, or responding that is outside of conscious awareness. This program was developed as an alternative to the Implicit Assessment Test (IAT) which was developed with social psychology as a way to assess implicit biases. While the IAT requires participants to categorize lists of words, the IRAP is designed to assess relational responses using contextual cues of interest. There are numerous studies that have used the IRAP, compared its results to that of (explicit) self-reported beliefs, and some that have compared it directly to the IAT. See the Empirical Support page for a detailed list of studies.

admin

Ole Miss IRAP software

Ole Miss IRAP software

Below is a zip file containing the Ole Miss IRAP (version 3.5). The program is easy to learn and readily adapted for your own research interests. The pdf file is the current manual for installing and using the program. I recommend reading this manual before anything else (it's only six pages). In addition to installation instructions and explanations of the program features, it also contains some useful information about handling participants and accessing data.

While the Ole Miss IRAP is copyrighted, this was done to keep it freely available. I encourage researchers to use, distribute, and modify the program as they see fit. Feel free to contact me if you encounter technical problems installing or using the program, or even if you would just like to bounce a study idea off of me. I'm all about the research and will do what I can to help.

Have fun!

Chad Drake

Ole Miss Matching-to-Sample software

Ole Miss Matching-to-Sample software

The lab at the University of Mississippi has made software available for the traditional matching-to-sample procedure. Below is an attachment containing a Visual Basic.NET program for a three-member, three class procedure. You will need to be logged into the ACBS website as a member in order to see and download the attachment.

Chad Drake

PsyScope: (Mac only) Easy-to-Use Software for Running Computer-Controlled Experiments

PsyScope: (Mac only) Easy-to-Use Software for Running Computer-Controlled Experiments

PsyScope: Easy-to-Use Software for Running Computer-Controlled Experiments in Psychology

PsyScope is for the Macintosh Platform only but several FREE Apple Macintosh emulators are available for download on line. PsyScope (Cohen, MacWhinney, Flatt, & Provost, 1993) is a user-friendly freeware Macintosh application which undergoes on-going development by psychologists for psychologists. It is the easiest to use and most versatile experiment generation software available, and it’s free.

PsyScope allows researchers to design complex psychology experiments without the need for programming skills. As the user constructs spider diagrams in a graphic interface using a limited number of graphic tools, PsyScope writes a scripting file which can be accessed directly by users familiar with scripting.

admin

Relational Completion Procedure (Dymond & Whelan, 2010)

Relational Completion Procedure (Dymond & Whelan, 2010)

An alternative to match-to-sample procedures, the Relational Completion Procedure is available below. 

The article is available in the publications section of the site, and by clicking directly here

The "Relational Completion Procedure.zip" file below is the more recent of the two files. Updates may be available at Simon Dymond's website as well.

Jen Plumb

Training and Assessment of Relational Precursors and Abilities (TARPA)

Training and Assessment of Relational Precursors and Abilities (TARPA)

Understanding and accounting for the ability to produce and understand completely novel sentences--accounting for the generativity of language--is critical to any account of language development (Malott, 2003), and to the creation of programs for teaching flexible and fully functional language repertoires.

RFT explains linguistic generativity in terms of learned contextually controlled relational responding referred to as relational framing. Typically developing children learn relational framing through natural language interactions during which they are exposed to contingencies that establish these response patterns (e.g., Lipkens, Hayes & Hayes, 1993; Luciano, Gómez & Rodríguez, 2007). However, children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) do not easily learn this key form of responding (e.g., Rehfeldt, Dillen, Ziomek, & Kowalchuk, 2007). Nonetheless, they can benefit from training of this repertoire (e.g., Murphy & Barnes-Holmes, 2009).

The Training & Assessment of Relational Precursors & Abilities (TARPA) is a recently developed computer-based protocol for the assessment of a progression of key domains of responding critical to the development of generative language. The TARPA is comprised of ten stages as follows: (i) basic discrimination; (ii) conditional discrimination involving similarity; (iii) conditional discrimination involving non-similarity (2 comparisons); (iv) conditional discriminations involving non-similarity (3 comparisons); (v) mutually entailed relational responding [e.g., deriving the symmetrical relation B --> A from the trained relation A --> B] (2 comparisons); (vi) mutually entailed relational responding (3 comparisons); (vii) combinatorial entailed relational responding [e.g., deriving the combinatorial relations A --> C and C --> A when trained with A--> B and B--> C] (2 comparisons); (viii) transfer of function [responding to a stimulus in a new and appropriate way based on it’s participation in a derived sameness relation] (2 comparisons); (ix) combinatorial entailed relational responding (3 comparisons); (x) transfer of function (3 comparisons). Each stage is further subdivided into multiple levels, and in the stages assessing derived relations (i.e., Stages 5-10), levels are subdivided into training sections and derivation sections.

A preliminary version of the TARPA has been correlated with the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale (VABS; Sparrow, Cicchetti & Balla, 2005). Currently ongoing research is using the most up-to-date version to assess the emergence of relational responding with typically developing children and children with autism in order to correlate performance on this protocol with level of functioning as assessed using standardized measures of language and cognition (e.g., PLS-4; Zimmerman, Steiner & Pond, 2002) as well as to gain some insight into the hierarchical structuring and other features of the protocol to aid its further development and refinement. For access to the TARPA and the TARPA manual, please email Siri Ming at siri@siriming.com

REFERENCES

  • Lipkens, R., Hayes, S.C., & Hayes, L.J. (1993). Longitudinal study of the development of derived relations in an infant. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 201-239.
  • Luciano, C., Gomez Becerra, I. & Rodriguez Valverde, M. (2007). The role of multiple- exemplar training and naming in establishing derived equivalence in an infant. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87, 349-365.
  • Malott, R. W. (2003). Behavior analysis and linguistic productivity. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 19, 11-18.
  • Murphy, C. & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2009). Derived more-less relational mands in children diagnosed with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 253–268.
  • Rehfeldt, R. A., Dillen, J. E., Ziomek, M. M., & Kowalchuk, R. E. (2007). Assessing relational learning deficits in perspective-taking in children with high functioning autism spectrum disorder. The Psychological Record, 57, 23-47.
  • Sparrow S. S., Cicchetti D.V & Balla D. A. (2005). Vineland II Adaptive Behavior Scales. (2nd ed.) American Guidance Service, Inc., Circle Pines, MN.
  • Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (2002). Preschool language scale-4. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
Ian Stewart

Training relational operants

Training relational operants

Below and attached includes a listserv discussion on training relational operants. 

 

THIS IS A DISCUSSION FROM A POST BY DARIN CAIRNS ON THE RFT LISTSERVE (I HAVE ATTACHED IT AS A WORD DOCUMENT AS WELL IN CASE THE FORMATTING IS TOUGH TO READ HERE.)

 

Hi there all I’m currently putting a curriculum together for working with children with Autism and have been getting remarkable success using exemplar based relational frame procedures.
Currently we are;
1) Using exemplar training (altering certain elements to highlight distinctions etc and using familiar labels etc – eg for Dad we’ll use child’s Dad’s name first then moving to less familiar names)
2) targeting specific frames - mainly hierarchical and didactic
3) using specified entailment and transformation of functions as performance goals
4) Using mastery criteria is still hard to pin but functionality is the goal anyway so we’re using RESA(A); Retention, Endurance, Stability and Application (we view the Entailment as Adductions) – for those familiar this from the Precision Teaching guys (sans the response rate at this stage – but very soon will have that).
We are messing around with prompting strats, errorless learning preparations, different ways to present the exemplars to vary the ease of acquisition of the response and then moving to framing (not sure if this is right as over selectivity may actually be enhanced and framing hindered), also looking at how to present exemplars in different orders to make the relational aspect more apparent etc etc etc….
My questions for the list are;
a) Other than coordination which seems to have to come first; do people have any thoughts on the order frames should be taught in?
b) Has anyone studied how one frame impacts upon another frame?
c) Is anyone aware of what sort of exemplars are better for teaching relational frames
d) Has anyone studied how the acquisition of a set of frames (or just a specific one) facilitates a learners ability to move through a curriculum or other tests eg how the acquisition of condionality and causality impacts on problem solving in general)
e) Has anyone studied how to ‘pull part’ relational networks (ie to identify how a person is making reasoning errors from an RFT perspective we could identify the required frames and then assess if they are functioning for the learner/performer)
Lots of questions. Any thoughts, references, ideas would be much appreciated.
All the best (and congrats on the Time article Steve – I think it will resonate with many by being presented the way it was, and engender curiosity even from those who thought it was ‘kooky’)
D
Darin Cairns
Clinical Psychologist Supervisor
Country Autism Services
Country Services Coordination
Hi Darin -- the attached three articles are relevant to questions a, b, c,
and e (nothing on d yet). Ian Stewart, John McElwee, Yvonne and I have
been looking at broadly similar issues -as those below, but in the context
of producing relevant educational software. Ian and John ran a workshop at
last year's ABA.

D.

Darin,

As Dermot mentioned in his post, myself, John McElwee, Dermot and Yvonne
put together a protocol for training up arbitarily applicable relational
responding in learning disabled children, which we've been refining for
the last year or so. This protocol is computer-based, and, drawing on a
database of electronic pictures and sounds, it allows a teacher to lead
a child through various stages which train and test mutual and
combinatorial entailment via multiple exemplars.

John and myself have been involved in workshops on Early Intensive
Behavioral Intervention and RFT the previous few ABAs and this summer
we're giving a similar one with the same theme - demonstrating to EIBI
people the effectiveness of RFT-type interventions for training language
generativity in learning disabled children. This summers workshop we'll
be discussing the extension of the original computer protocol via the
creation of a protocol that trains and tests relational pre-cursors from
simple dicriminations, through non arbitrary relational conditional
discriminations, through arbitrary relational conditional
discriminations. We'll also be discussing how this work relates to the
ABLA, and the ABLLS early learning protocols, and more specifically, how
RFT predicts the type of patterns that are seen in terms of the
correlations between ABLA and stimulus equivalence and language
performance. We are currently writing a paper on the latter and applying
for funding to test specific RFT-based predictions with regard to the
difficulty of various stages of the ABLA.

Your questions are very important ones. In our RFT-based analysis of the
ABLSS curriculum we are stressing the importance of the order of
training particular frames and how particular types of frames are indeed
necessary precursors of others. For example, flexible contextual control
over same/different responding may be an important precursor of
hierarchy. If you or others are interested I can send you past ABA
workshop .ppt presentations on EIBI and RFT.

Best,

Ian.

Hey Darin;

I think the work you are doing is important. In terms of the
sequence of which frames should be taught first, I am not sure the
data is there to guide us on this issue. Thus, put on your
clinicians hat and monkey around. Choose a frame that will have the
greatest impact for the child in his or her environment. In other
words, what frames will the child's environment immediately support
and what frames, once established will be of most use for that child
to more effective participate and operate in his/her environemnt.
This is a logical exercise. But, I suspect that a) the logic will
steer you right in many cases & b) you will hit walls that will
require troubleshooting. These successes and temporary walls are
perhaps the most important bits of information for the field. Keep
us posted so the basic minded folks can help work out the issues
that will assuredly crop up.

Question: Do have non-arbitrary tasks built into your training
sequences? Whether these are absolutely necessary pre-requisites is
still uncertain. Several studies they have definitely shown to
facilitative or the development of arbitrarily applicable derived
relational responding. But I suspect when working with language
delayed indivduals non-arbitrary tasks will be particularily useful
in making the targeted relation more salient. There are a lot of
good old studies (50'-70's) showing how MET can facilitate what's
been labeled transposition. Which is, crudely put, responding in
terms of a relation and not an absolute value.

TONY;
I agree the mistakes may be very enlightening. Again, I think the
early transposition literature sheds light on the issue of these
mistakes and implicitly (not explicity because authors often
relegate these mistakes and sucesses to the realm of schemas or
language as mediator) demonstrates ways to program training
materials such as to promote derived relational responding.

I agree that errorless procedures are often times great remedies. I
found it VERY difficult, during my pilot work for my thesis that
when dealing with derived relational responding, it is very hard to
get kids ("typically developing") to "get" the target relation and
not some other absolute property of the stimulus conditions.

FINAL NOTE (I half-heartedly promise): Would it be alright if I
posted these contributions to this thread on the RFT list to the
education section of contextualpsychology.com


Hi Nick hows it going?
I think it would be fine to put this on the education list. The more ideas and insight the better.
Choose a frame that has greatest impact for the child;
1) To me Baer’s behavioural cusp concept applies here. But it’s interesting to consider how one identifies a required frame. We could do an example of cusp analysis with frames.
2) Cusps also apply to ‘bang for your buck’ in terms of what the curriculum sequence will benefit from the most. Ie the PT guys look at tool skills to promote generative learning. I wonder what the ‘tool’ frames are in a given curriculum context?
Do we have non arbitrary tasks built in to help language delayed individuals target relation more salient?
1) We’re messing around with it but this is definitely something that interests me. If I’m interpreting you correctly, what I can feedback at this point is that the non-arbitrary tasks have proven the fastest road to relational framing for the learner. We kinda think of it as a sort of ‘within stimulus’ prompt as the answer lay in explicit previous learning and then we lead them to more purely relational situations where they must entail. When we don’t prep this way the children become anxious and start guessing.
2) Can you throw me some refs? Interested in the MTS examples you are referring to.
3) The issue of protecting the learner from over-selectivity pops up at this point too as the non arb stuff can play right into the hands of non relational behaviour being reinforced at the expense of relational…
Mistakes…
1) Have been fascinating to watch and have directed our approach with the learner quite a lot.
2) I wonder if IRAP procedures can be used here to give us insights into the relevant networks and construct the instructional design from there…?
3) What errorless learning strats (ie prompts) did you try in your thesis Nick? It’s a real struggle here too. Non arb being the only real strat we have (I’m wary of visuals). We have tried to conmstruct a hierarchy based on how previous learning will support the correct answer (ie non arb), not impact directly on framing (ie we use hypothetical examples), to impacting against framing relationally (Ie use examples where the entailment is logically true but experientially wrong – ie the child has to ‘trust’ the entailment not his explicit learning history)
Here’s a question. If we forget the order issue what is the process (I have avoided the word mechanism ) by which a frame effects another frame? What is necessary for a frame to ‘bump’ into another frame and then change it? This will go a long way to helping us understand generative learning and adductions I’d think. Real bang for your buck instructional designing.
D
Darin Cairns
Clinical Psychologist Supervisor
Country Services Coordination

The cusp concept looks like another way of saying “increases psychological flexibility” (“behavioral cusp” is in a frame of coordination with “increases psychological flexibility”?)
Re: “Here’s a question. If we forget the order issue what is the process (I have avoided the word mechanism ) by which a frame effects another frame? What is necessary for a frame to ‘bump’ into another frame and then change it?”
Along the lines of a comment in Ian’s earlier post, it seems to me that coordination and difference are essential for hierarchy. In broader RFT/ACT terms, once coordination and difference are established – which respectively have the most precision and the broadest scope, hierarchical relations serve to attenuate/balance precision and scope in context - and thus to increase behavioral flexibility. In other words, frames don’t bump into other frames unless/until we relate relations….relate multiple exemplars qua correspondence AND difference and you get hierarchy….
Once you get correspondence, difference, hierarchy, only then are more complex relations possible - deictic, conditional, and beyond…
Seems right
One thing
It seems to me that as soon as you have multiple frames you can
leverage training by demanding several simultaneously.
In other words, you can make them "bump into" each other
usefully
For example
Once a kid has coordination and difference, you can
a) use exclusion to train new material so that it simultaneously
strengthens frames of cooridination and differnce.
For example, saying "where's the gug-gub" in the presence of a
novel object and a ball (that is in a frame of coordination with "ball")
is a good way to have another "ball trial" AND distinguish balls and gub-gubs
AND train a frame of coordination between the novel object and "gub-gub."
b) similarly you can train using deliberate errors. Once the kid relaly knows Mommy, point to
Ma-Ma and say "there's Daddy" and pause. The kid will laugh or otherwise
indicate the error. "That right, That's not Daddy ... that's Mommy!"
You simultaneously apply a frame of coordination twice and a frame of distinction
c) similarly you can early on play with flexible contextual control.
(If Ma Ma was Da Da, where's Ma Ma?). This is cool because it simultaneously
strenghtens two instances of a FoC and a FoD, plus it makes the Crel control
more arbitrary
To do these kinds of things you need multiple frames ... and I think even in
the earliest training of FoC, it should be done along of training others frames.
it makes the relational feature more likely to be the invariant. For example,
why just train "Da Da" when you can also train "not Da Da."
Lots of discrete trail programs seem to ignore the benefits this can provide
- S
Steven C. Hayes

Right on! And... In defense of training just "da da." I think
there are two issues.
1) Thinking that way has not hit the main pipeline just yet. It is
out of the box. I do not believe that training "not da da" is in
most peoples realm of importance let alone possibility.
2) (this one may take me a while). Discrete trials are most commonly
used in the world ABA with autism. Initially, it is often difficult
to get any kind of vocal responding under stimulus control. With
some kids it is a real pain in the @** struggle.
As such, I think that a large part of the field has been or is
quite impressed by the POWER of reinforcement to train just about
behavior (yes yes not sophisticated language behavior like I am
displaying right here before your very eyes) that we just plug on to
see what else we can bring to strength. This is done, without
looking at the broader desired repertiore. Sometimes I feel that we
are engaging in a modern form of S-R psychology. Call it R-S
psychology. By the way this statement is not meant to indeminfy any
individual/s. As I have the utmost respect for the work I just
described.

 

Nick B
 

admin

Research Failures

Research Failures

It seems important to let the world know about RFT research failures or disconfirmations of the theory. If you have any, please add a daughter page and describe the study as well as you can to let the community know about it. If you have ideas about why it might have failed, feel free to list them. If you think that it did not work because RFT is incorrect, feel free to state that and to suggests necessary changes to the theory, both big and small.

If few pages are added, do not jump to the conclusion that there are few failures. This page is designed to invite them into the light but often researchers hold failures back.

To this date, I confess I know of no RFT failures. But if they are there, we want them to be known, and given good visibility here on the ACBS website.

Steven Hayes

Communication au symposium Pleine Conscience des 37èmes Journées Scientifiques de l'AFTCC, Paris 12 décembre 2009

Communication au symposium Pleine Conscience des 37èmes Journées Scientifiques de l'AFTCC, Paris 12 décembre 2009

Étude pilote ouverte d’un groupe de psychoéducation à la pleine conscience et l’acceptation à l’intention de personnes vivant avec un trouble bipolaire.

J. GRAND (1), B. PUTOIS (2), B. SCHOENDORFF (3)
(1) Psychothérapeute libérale, Lyon
(2) Université Louis Lumière, Lyon
(3) Université Claude Bernard, Lyon

Problématique:

Conception, mise en place et évaluation d’un programme de groupe ouvert de psychoéducation à la pleine conscience et l’acceptation pour personnes bipolaires. 

Méthode:

Étude pilote sous forme de groupe ouvert de psychoéducation (6 séances d’intervention et deux de suivi) au sein d’une association de personnes bipolaires à Lyon. 21 personnes ont participé, mais seulement 5 aux 4 points de mesures (7 à trois points). Du fait de la nature de groupe ouvert, le choix avait été fait de limiter les évaluations.
Seulement deux mesures ont été utilisées : le questionnaire d’Acceptation et d’Action-AAQ2 (Hayes 2004) et la « Cible des valeurs » (Lundgren 2005).

Résultats:

L’augmentation moyenne des scores AAQ n’était pas significative.
Les résultats au questionnaire de la cible des valeurs pré-post (N=7) montraient une augmentation des actions dans le domaine travail/formation (ANOVAChi² = 8,00 p=0,018; d=1,61), mais pas de différence au suivi à 6
mois (N=5). Il n’y avait pas de différence dans le domaine du soin physique personnel (sauf entre pré et mid,
N=7). Dans le domaine des relations, il y avait une différence significative (N=5) entre post et suivi à 6 mois
(p=0,04). Globalement, il y avait un effet tendanciel pré-post sur la cible des valeurs (p=0,063).
Cette intervention n’a donc pas eu d’effet sur la capacité d’acceptation des participants ni leurs actions en
direction de leurs valeurs dans le domaine des loisirs, elle n’a eu d’effet sur les actions dans le domaine des soins
personnels qu’au début de l’intervention (pré-mid), et dans le domaine du travail qu’entre le début et la fin de
l’intervention (pré-post). Ces effets n’étaient pas maintenus au suivi. Enfin, pour 5 sujets, il y avait un effet sur
les relations entre la fin du traitement et le suivi à 6 mois.

Discussion:

Cette étude pilote suggère que la forme de groupe ouvert choisie n’est pas une façon adaptée de délivrer l’ACT à
une population bipolaire. Nous avons tenu à faire connaître ces résultats négatifs et proposerons quelques pistes
pour une meilleure adaptation d’un traitement, qui semble donner de bons résultats dans notre pratique
individuelle chez cette population.

benjamin schoendorff

Research Predictions

Research Predictions

Please add a child page describing any predictions you may have based on RFT. You should state the reasons for the prediction clearly, and be sure to leave your name.

By putting it on the page you are giving away the idea -- anyone is free to test it. However, we would ask if anyone does that, they ask the individual if they want to be acknowledged in the article that may result (not necesarily as an author, but perhaps in a footnote ... such as "The core ideas tested in this article was first suggested to us by Bessy Bluebottom, and we would like to thank her for the suggestion." Something like that.)

Steven Hayes

IRAP

IRAP

Some theoretical questions and question-hypotheses concerning the IRAP and RFT:

The IRAP seems to be a measure of implicit preferences, in contrast with the explicit preferences. Explicit preferences are more influenced by social control (eg political correctness).

1. Does relational framing influence the more implicit preferences as well as the explicit? And if yes, how?

2. Can we say it’s good to be aware of our implicit preferences (as revealed by the IRAP) and make choices without bringing them into account? The implicit is good to know, to realize it’s there. But the explicit is the more important? We can learn to live our life in the direction of our explicit preferences. E.g.: ‘Muslims are terrorists’ vs ‘I want to live with all kinds of people. Not all Muslims are equal. I do respect them.’ IRAP might reveal the first relation, but the second could be more important.

3. When existing relational networks are extended with new S, will these S influence the implicit functions or the explicit or both (depending on context)?

4. when the implicit preferences and the explicit preferences are different, contextual influences are responsible for these differences? Experiential avoidance, political correctness, … If these contextual S are not present, the implicit and explicit preferences are growing more toward each other?

5. cognitive therapy is working on the explicit relations by social control? After cognitive therapy the implicit positions might stay unchanged?

6. when the social pressure is very high (IRAP on ‘Muslims are terrorists’ taken by a clearly Muslim researcher and without anonymity) even the implicit measures could be influenced? (contextual cues are stronger).

7. explicit, but perhaps also implicit preferences can reverse? E.g. the Muslim-experiment described above: when after a while the apparently-Muslim researcher says he’s anti-Muslim (eg political refugee) – after this the IRAP-scores might reverse?

Francis De Groot

francis.de.groot@fracarita.org

Francis De Groot

RFT and Basic Social Research

RFT and Basic Social Research

First, I would like to say that, from my reading of Part I. in the RFT book, it seems the overall program of research has a stable foothold within behavior analysis. This is further accentuated by the fact that applied research is beginning to appear in the Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis (see Ninness et al.)in which RFT principles are built into a computer program which teaches trigonomic functions and their graphical representations in an efficient manner. Believe me, it works...I am a co-author on the latest of these RFT/math studies, and, coming into the project, I knew pretty much nothing about functions and their graphical representations but after approx. an hour in the program as a pilot subject I had a firm grasp on the concepts...as well as many novel formulae and graphs never before seen.

Because work like this is emerging along side a strong basic research program, I think RFT is here to stay. As for its future, I have been working on a way to study meta/macrocontingencies and cultural materialism in a basic laboratory setting. This area is the domain of "cultural analysis," or "behavioral anthropology," and even crosses over into OBM. When studied behavior-analytically the phrase "culture" is really synonymous with "social behavior."

If this basic research catches on, I predict the future could see a merging of both research programs in order to study how relational framing operates in relation to social behavior. The beauty about this particular social-behavioral research program is that it begins (presumably) in a laboratory setting analogous to the most basic contexts which give rise to interlocking contingencies as they occur in nature, and as they (presumably) occurred in the evolution of cultures. So, this program would (presumably) be the most thourough, inductive, investigation of social behavior to-date. This is similar to the beginnings of the behavior-analytic research program in general: Skinner started with the most basic contexts and slowly built upon them until now, where we can study language and cognition, and do so in an inductive, non-hypothetical, manner.

Perhaps combining RFT with such research could reveal principles relevant to symbolic behavior, myths, taboos, and "norms" etc... but would do so with the precision of a basic behavioral laboratory. Other basic researchers are already hard at work developing an equation of choice behavior, perhaps a few decades will reveal equations of norms, taboos, and even a "terrorism equation." It seems pretty far-fetched, but what an exciting way to earn a living!

Shawn Boles added on 4/25/2006:

Tony Biglan has written on this as well.

Changing Cultural Practices

Hardcover: 464 pages
Publisher: Context Press (January 1, 1995)
Language: English
ISBN: 1878978225

Todd A. Ward

RFT and magical thinking in childhood

RFT and magical thinking in childhood

RFT and magical thinking: hypothesis

A hypothesis: is it possible that the period of ‘magical thinking’ in the development of children (enduring until adulthood) is depending on the development of processes central in RFT: developing of mutual entailment, literality of thoughts, reason giving and causal thinking? When children are developing those skills, but aren’t fully acquainted with them, they might more easily fall in the traps of magical thinking.

Possible test: children who are later in developing those skills, should show delayed magical thinking too (and vice versa).

Francis De Groot

francis.de.groot@fracarita.org

Francis De Groot

Whole Lotta Predictions

Whole Lotta Predictions

I challenged the RFT list serve to come up with some good solid predictions that went beyond the several dozen in the RFT book.

This list, raw and unfiltered, is the result. Some of these ideas are great. Some seem off. And anyone was and is allowed to play. But it seemed more important to get people thinking than to get it right if "right" meant that some "leader" says "this is right."

If you have ideas, back up to the next highest level and add a child page and put yours out there!

- S

Steve Hayes

Predictions from Steve

  • Responding in accordance with a coherent relational network will take less time (on average) than responding in accordance with an incoherent network (subject, of course, to the usual caveats concerning individual histories).
  • Relating derived relations will produce some of the same effects that have been observed for analogical reasoning
  • RFT models of semantic relations, analogy, executive function tasks, perspective-taking and the like should produce neural effects that overlap to some degree with the effects observed in the mainstream neuro-cog literature.
  • Increasing the extent, flexibility, and fluency of relational frames, relational networks, relating relations, relating relational networks, the transformation of functions, and contextual control over each of these, should impact positively on a variety of standard measures of human language and cognition.

Steve posted a list of new things RFT does to the Academy of Cognitive Therapy
June 2005. The list was:

RFT:

  • Provides new ways to do language training
  • Has lead to a new and increasingly empirically supported psychotherapy
    (ACT) and to quite number of new psychotherapy techniques
  • Suggests how to establish a sense of self in children
  • Shows some of how to train children in "theory of mind"
  • Gives a process account of mindfulness
  • Predicts how many basic cognitive skills form
  • Predicts new ways to increase openness to new learning
  • Explains some of where psychological rigidity comes from
  • Leads to a new model of psychopathology
  • Suggests some of the core skills involves in language and its subskills such as analogy and metaphor
  • Shows why existing information processing research in specific areas (e.g., analogy) is flawed and show how to correct that flaw
  • Predicts new methods how to increase some intellectual abilities
  • Predicts new methods for how to increase motivation verbally
  • Predicts some new methods to decrease motivation verbally
  • Has lead to new ways we might assess current cognitive relations
  • Explains some of why cognitive fusion emerges, why it is harmful, and what to do about it
  • Explains some of why experiential avoidance emerges, why it is harmful, and what to do about it
  • Provides unexpected predictions about neurobiological responses to specific cognitive tasks

----------

What happens to Crel and Cfun in RFT studies when you teach folks to apply defusion during testing, and or when you teach defusion, train, and then test? I am thinking of M Dougher's recent study with > or < relations with shock. I wonder whether defusion would alter the transformation, perhaps leading subjects to not rip off the shock electrodes in the context of > relation. I wonder whether defusion would strengthen or perhaps weaken Crel and/or Cfun. My guess is that it may result in more rapid learning of Crel, but knock out Cfun. This would be cool to show. Maybe someone has done this, but if not we really should cook up some experiments along these lines.

-j forsyth

------------

1. Additional corollary hypotheses:
(A) Speed of acquisition of AARR during an REP task (i.e., number of trials needed to respond consistently correctly) will correlate significantly and inversely with verbal IQ. (can’t recall off hand if Denis O’Hora has already tested this specifically yet).
(B) This one would be a doozie to quantify and test, but it follows from RFT: Subjects presented with a novel metaphor who generate higher numbers of apt comparisons (especially in shorter amounts of time) will perform better (i.e., will respond correctly more frequently and given less training trials) in an REP task that assesses their ability to correctly derive relations after two previously trained frames are brought into coordination.
2. Additional corollary hypotheses:
(A) AARR in fully verbal subjects will fail to occur over time within an experimental context, given a consistent lack of reinforcement for AARR and/or consistent punishment of AARR within that context.
3. Additional corollary hypotheses:
(A) The same established verbal relation (e.g., A is similar to B, which is similar to C) can be shown to accompany different functional transformations across different experimental contexts.
(B) Identical functional transformations can be shown to be achieved through the training of different verbal relations.

J T

---------------

read some RFT-research on the change of psychological function of stimulus C by putting it in relation with A-B (sexual excitement, taste preference, mood). What if C is relationally framed with 2 different classes: A-B-C, and X-Y-C. And let's say A is experienced a bit negative, and X also a bit negative. Would C become experienced more negative, than when it's framed with only one class? This might be an operationalisation of multiple small life experiences leading to a larger reaction.

De Groot, Francis [francis.de.groot@fracarita.org]

Steven Hayes

The first papers on RFT

The first papers on RFT

The origins of RFT are described in the "personal prologue" to the first book on RFT:

Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational Frame Theory: A Post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Plenum Press. 

That prologue is attached below.

From about 1981 thru early 1986 Steve Hayes and Aaron Brownstein, a well known basic behavior analyst, ran their labs jointly together at the University of North Carolina, Greensboro. Despite the fact that Hayes was a clinical psychologist and Brownstein was a basic behavior analyst the team was highly cohesive and productive. While Steve was trying to develop an analysis of verbal rules and verbal meaning Aaron suggested he consider the relatively recent and not well known phenomenon of stimulus equivalence. Hayes reports being shocked by the phenomenon and the absence of a process-oriented account of it. Unwilling to leave it as a mere outcome he reports spending almost an entire week pondering the processes that could possibly give rise to equivalence. In a kind of "lightbulb moment" he came to the simple idea that relating is likely an instance of generalized instrumental learning (that is, an operant), much as with generalized imitation. 

Hayes excitedly presented his analysis to Brownstein, who thought the idea was plausible and very interesting. The two soon laid out nearly 20 studies that would test the idea. That list has been lost to history, but Hayes claims that when he last saw it in the early 2000's nearly every study had in fact been done and all were supportive. 

The first paper that began to lay out the ideas behind RFT (and ACT and Functional Contextualism for that matter) was this one: Hayes, S. C. (1984). Making sense of spirituality. Behaviorism, 12, 99-110. It is attached below. That paper was already being written in 1983 and the lab worked on these ideas for months if not years so RFT would have to have begun somewhere in the 1981 thru 1983 period. Precisely when has been lost to history.

The first full presentation of the model was in an invited address co-authored with Aaron Brownstein, : Hayes, S. C. & Brownstein, A. J. (May 1985). Verbal behavior, equivalence classes, and rules: New definitions, data, and directions. Invited address presented at the meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Columbus, OH.

A jointly authored Hayes and Brownstein paper was drafted before that talk but publication was delayed because Hayes had accepted a job at the University of Nevada, Reno as Director of Clinical Training and was busy planning the move. The paper was still being rewritten for submission for publication when Aaron died unexpectedly while dancing with his wife, on April 12, 1986. It is attached below in two sections because the file to too large to attach it as one. 

Brownstein was a better known figure at the time and Hayes felt that it was not right to assume that Aaron would have approved of the details of the analysis. He also feared that others might think we was using Aaron's reputation to get attention to the piece. Thus he continued to revise the paper. It was delivered in revised form in June of that year in this paper: Hayes, S. C. (June 1986). What is a verbal stimulus? Invited address presented to the Summer Institute on Verbal Relations, Behaviorists European Summer Academy, Bad Kreuznach, West Germany.  The proceeding of that conferences were submitted for publication sometime in 1987 but due to extreme delays in publication by a skeptical publisher Hayes and his new wife Linda Parrot Hayes eventually started a new publishing company, Context Press, in large part so as to finally bring that volume into print. The proceedings appeared in 1991in the first volume produced by Context Press: Hayes, S. C. (1991). A relational control theory of stimulus equivalence. In L. J. Hayes & P. N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogues on verbal behavior (pp. 19-40). Reno, NV: Context Press. A footnote to that chapter acknowledged it great debt to Aaron and had he lived people would likely think of him as a co-founder of RFT.

Parenthetically, Context Press continued to grow and was eventually purchased by New Harbinger Publications and still exists today.

So where did RFT come from? Hayes summarizes his views in the personal prologue of the first RFT book this way: 

Where RFT came from? Putting together these sources of influence —radical functionalism, rigorous experimental orientation, the lead provided by rule-governance and simple equivalence, and the analytic creativity of basic behavior analysts —RFT is a fairly natural extension of the field itself as I was trained to view it. (Hayes, 2001, p. 9).

As a historical side note:

Hayes has long claimed that anyone who really understands generalized imitation understands the RFT idea that relating is an operant. Generalized imitation is an example of the kind of radical functionalism that is inside RFT. To understand generalized imitation, start with Don Baer, one of the founders of applied behavior analysis. Don's mentor was Jack Gewirtz who championed he idea of generalized imitation (Gewirtz, J. L., & Stingle, K. G. (1968). Learning of generalized imitation as the basis for identification. Psychological Review, 75(5), 374–397. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0026378). You can read about his background in a long oral interview done for the Society for Research in Child Development (https://www.srcd.org/sites/default/files/file-attachments/gewirtz_jacob_interview.pdf). Its a bit of a shock to realize that Jack Gewirtz was trained by Beth Wellman at the Iowa Child Welfare Station -- the heroic woman who almost single handedly stood up to the eugenic lies of Louis Terman when the eugenicists were claiming all intelligence is genetic. A good book on that horrible history is Marilyn Brookwood's "The Orphans of Davenport: Eugenics, the Great Depression, and the War over Children's Intelligence". Stated another way, RFT up with a rich functional behavioral tradition.

Linking RFT to that wing of behavioral thinking may also help make sense of some of the resistance to RFT ideas even today ... but that is a story still to be told.

Steven Hayes

RFT Books

RFT Books

There are many books to help you learn more about RFT.  

Click here for a list of RFT Books

Community

RFT Listserv

RFT Listserv

The RFT email listserv is a forum for scientists, scholars, practitioners, and students to discuss Relational Frame Theory (RFT), an explicitly psychological account of human language and cognition. RFT is an approach designed to be a pragmatically useful analysis of complex human behavior, providing empirical and conceptual tools to conduct an experimental analysis of substantive topics in this arena. It is based on the principles of behavior analysis and contextual behavioral science. RFT is looked to as the conceptual basis for Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, as well as an increasingly large array of other applied interventions such as language training program, or programs designed to develop a sense of self in developmentally delayed children.

The RFT listserv is open to professionals or graduate students in relevant fields. Paid ACBS membership (student or professional) is required to join the RFT Listserv.

Login to your ACBS account and click here to join the RFT listserv

Community